Ina
Zaimi
,
Amber J.
Dood
and
Ginger V.
Shultz
*
Department of Chemistry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109, USA. E-mail: gshultz@umich.edu
First published on 8th November 2023
Asking students to explain why phenomena occur at a molecular level is vital to increasing their understanding of chemistry concepts. One way to elicit students’ mechanistic reasoning and guide construction of knowledge is through Writing-to-Learn (WTL), which is a promising approach for students in organic chemistry courses. In the design of WTL assignments, rhetorical aspects provide an authentic context by designating a role, genre, and audience for students. This context can support students’ learning, but, if the rhetorical aspects misalign with the learning objectives of the assignment, they can hinder students’ ability to achieve these objectives. In this project, we designed a WTL assignment about a base-free Wittig reaction, which we implemented in an organic chemistry laboratory course. Here, we explore how changes in the rhetorical aspects of this assignment can influence the way students explain two different comparisons of reaction mechanisms. We consider how students use explicit and implicit properties and how the use of these features compares within the context of the reaction. Results indicate that, when the rhetorical aspects align with the learning objectives of the assignment, students provide more productive elaborations that use both explicit and implicit properties. This is supported by both the analysis of students’ writing and students’ feedback about the assignments.
It has been reported in several contexts that students can draw mechanistic arrows and describe how reaction mechanisms happen, but not explain the concepts behind the mechanistic arrows (Graulich, 2015; Dood and Watts, 2022, 2023). While students are consistently tasked to draw and describe reaction mechanisms, they are not always required to explain them or provide underlying reasoning for how and why the reaction mechanisms happen. Research recommends using writing to support students in practicing their mechanistic reasoning and their causal reasoning and, thus, in constructing a deep understanding of organic chemistry reactions (Dood and Watts, 2022, 2023; Finkenstaedt-Quinn et al., 2023).
Researchers have examined a variety of tasks designed to engage students in aspects of reasoning. They designed and studied short-response writing tasks and constructed-response questions (Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Dood et al., 2018; Dood et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood et al., 2020a, 2020b; Yik et al., 2021). For example, Cooper et al. (2016) designed a writing task on acid–base reactions where students drew the reaction mechanisms and were asked “how” and “why” the reaction mechanisms happen. Cooper et al. (2016) determined that students provided more elaborated, more detailed explanations when they asked students the questions separately, rather than jointly. Subsequent research has elicited students’ reasoning by using these types of constructed-response questions (Crandell et al., 2018, Dood et al., 2018, Dood et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood et al., 2020a, 2020b; Yik et al., 2021).
Additionally, researchers recommended using case-comparison questions (Graulich and Schween, 2018; Caspari and Graulich, 2019; Graulich et al., 2019; Graulich and Caspari, 2020; Rodemer et al., 2020; Watts et al., 2021). For example, Graulich et al. (2019) designed a writing task on substitution reactions where students selected two molecules with similar reactivities out of three molecules. While Graulich et al. (2019) did not analyse for mechanistic explanations and causal explanations, they did analyse for explicit properties and implicit properties, which compose causal explanations. In comparison to students who identified explicit properties, students who identified implicit properties tended to select the correct two molecules (Graulich et al., 2019). Prior and subsequent research has elicited students’ reasoning by using these case-comparison questions (Graulich and Schween, 2018; Caspari and Graulich, 2019; Graulich and Caspari, 2020; Rodemer et al., 2020; Watts et al., 2021).
Alternatively, researchers have designed long-response writing assignments, and they have recommended using Writing-to-Learn (WTL) assignments (Schmidt-Mccormack et al., 2019; Watts et al., 2020; Brandfonbrener et al., 2021). Research has shown that WTL assignments support students’ understanding, specifically their understanding of acids and bases (Schmidt-Mccormack et al., 2019), their understanding of nucleophiles and electrophiles (Watts et al., 2020), and their understanding of resonance structures (Brandfonbrener et al., 2021). Moreover, research has suggested that WTL assignments support students’ reasoning about reaction mechanisms (Watts et al., 2020). For example, Watts et al. (2020) designed a WTL assignment on acid–base reactions where students explained reaction mechanisms. While Watts et al. (2020) did not analyse for mechanistic explanations and causal explanations, they did analyse for entities, which compose mechanistic explanations, and properties, which compose causal explanations. All students identified entities, and most students identified properties (Watts et al., 2020). This finding suggests that subsequent research is required to explore how WTL assignments elicit students’ reasoning.
Inclusion of rhetorical aspects in a WTL assignment is well documented as a best practice (Rivard, 1994; Bangert-Drowns et al., 2004; Anderson et al., 2015; Gere et al., 2018, 2019; Finkenstaedt-Quinn et al., 2021a, 2023). WTL assignments provide a specified audience because this elicits explanations based on assumptions students hold about the audience (Hand et al., 2004; Chen, 2013; Chen et al., 2016). However, students are still cognizant of the instructor and may address the instructor in their writing as well as the specified audience. For example, Gere et al. (2018) found that students’ explanations changed if students addressed the audience more than the instructor, if students addressed the instructor more than the audience, or if the students balanced between audiences. Sometimes, the audience (e.g., a statistical consultant) and the instructor had shared knowledge, and this encouraged students’ explanations (Gere et al., 2018). Other times, the audience (e.g., students’ grandparents) and the instructor had unshared knowledge, and this constrained students’ explanations (Gere et al., 2018). Gupte et al. (2021) echoed this finding. These findings suggest that subsequent research is required to explore how WTL assignments’ rhetorical aspects shape students’ reasoning. Therefore, in response, this study explores not only how a WTL assignment elicits students’ reasoning but also how its rhetorical aspects shape students’ reasoning.
Succinctly, we designed the Wittig WTL assignment where students explained a traditional intermolecular Wittig reaction mechanism, explained a modified intramolecular Wittig reaction mechanism (Schirmer et al., 2015; Grandane et al., 2019), and compared them. We redesigned the writing assignment, keeping its learning objective but changing its rhetorical aspects. Herein, we explore how students' writing changed when the writing assignment's rhetorical aspects changed.
Cognitively, this theory defines the writing process as a recursive, cyclic process (Fig. 1) (Emig, 1971, 1977; Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984). First, students plan. Second, students write, or transform internal representations of knowledge (i.e., thinking) into external representations of knowledge (i.e., writing). As delineated by this theory, students’ writing captures their thinking, and, specifically, organic chemistry students’ writing captures their reasoning, as demonstrated by Watts et al. (2020). Third, students review, which moves them on or moves them into planning and writing. This theory informed the implementation of the Wittig WTL assignment, where students planned and wrote first drafts, provided and received peer reviews, and reviewed and wrote final drafts. Consequently, this theory informed our collection of students’ final drafts. Students decided to include – or not to include – concepts from their first drafts to their final drafts in the peer-review process. Watts et al. (2020) asserted that students’ final drafts “best captures the features they found important to include.” Therefore, students’ final drafts should capture more complete reasoning than their first drafts would.
Socially, this theory defines a writing assignment as a “rhetorical problem” with its learning objectives and its rhetorical aspects (i.e., genre, audience, and role) and a writing response as a “rhetorical solution” (Fig. 1) (Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984). Hayes (1996) proposed that students’ motivations mediate between the two. Accordingly, students plan, write, and review in response to the writing assignment's learning objectives (e.g., Should I include this concept?), the writing assignment's rhetorical aspects (e.g., Does my audience know this concept? Should I explain this concept?), and their motivations (e.g., Do I want to include this concept? Do I want to explain this concept?). This theory informed our design of the Wittig WTL assignment and our redesigns of its rhetorical aspects, intending to align problem and solution. Therefore, one assignment's rhetorical aspects should shape more complete reasoning than another assignment's rhetorical aspects would, if the assignment addresses the problem or if students’ motivation does.
Finally, this theory informed our assessment of the Wittig WTL assignment. We assessed an “effective” assignment as one that effectively aligned the problem, determined by the assignment's learning objective and rhetorical aspects, and the solution, demonstrated by students’ final drafts.
Adopting the codes required exemplifying them in regard to the context, the base-free Wittig reaction. Graulich et al. (2019) presented two explicit codes: the explicit-descriptive code and the explicit-functional code. We exemplified the explicit-descriptive code, where students stated explicit structures (e.g., an ester group) and explicit properties (e.g., a negative charge) (Table 1). Additionally, we exemplified the explicit-functional code, where students placed explicit structures and explicit properties within a context (e.g., the base-free Wittig reaction) and provided them with a function (e.g., acting as a base) (Table 1). Graulich et al. (2019) also presented two implicit codes: the implicit-descriptive code and the implicit-functional code. We exemplified the implicit-descriptive code, where students stated implicit properties (e.g., resonance effects) (Table 1). Additionally, we exemplified the implicit-functional code, where students placed implicit properties within a context (e.g., the base-free Wittig reaction) and provided them with a function (e.g., acting as a base) (Table 1).
Code | Graulich et al.'s (2019) definition | Our definition | Examples |
---|---|---|---|
Explicit descriptive | The student states an explicit property of the molecules. | The student states an explicit structure (e.g., the hydrogen atom, the oxygen atom, the alkene group, the ester group, a resonance structure or a resonance contributor, and a conjugated structure), or the student states an explicit property (e.g., an electron source, an electron sink, the charge of an atom, the symmetry of a group, the acidity of a molecule, the basicity of a molecule, or the stability of a molecule). | “The starting materials are different by having a ketone [in Scheme 1] and an ester [in Scheme 2].” |
“Acrylate [in Scheme 3] has one CO 2 Et, compared to the two [CO 2 Ets] that maleate [in Scheme 2] has.” | |||
Explicit functional | The student states the role of the entity in the problem context, or the student states an explicit property and the problem context. | The student states an explicit structure or an explicit property, and the student provides the explicit structure or the explicit property with a role (e.g., acting as an electrophile, acting as a nucleophile, causing stability, increasing favourability, or increasing likelihood) within the base-free Wittig reaction. | “[In Scheme 2], an external base is not required, and the deprotonation occurs intramolecularly.” |
“[In Scheme 3], this is not possible because the [acrylate] does not have carbon–oxygen double-bonds to push electrons to.” | |||
Implicit descriptive | The student states an implicit property of the molecules, or the student adds an implicit property to an explicit-functional elaboration. | The student states an implicit property (e.g., the size of an atom, the electronegativity of an atom, the dipole of a bond, the partial charges from the dipole of a bond, the delocalization of electrons in resonance, the conjugation of electrons in resonance, the resonance effects of a group, the induction effects of a group, the electrophilicity of a molecule, the nucleophilicity of a molecule, the acidity of a molecule, the basicity of a molecule, the pKa of a molecule, or the stability of a molecule), or the student adds an implicit property to an explicit-functional elaboration. | “[In Scheme 2], these features include resonance and the presence of electron withdrawing groups.” |
“[In Scheme 3], acrylate does not have this resonance stabilization since it only has one carbonyl group [instead of two carbonyl groups that malate has].” | |||
Implicit functional | The student states an implicit property and refers to its role in the problem context. | The student states an implicit property, and the student provides the implicit property with a role (e.g., acting as an electrophile, acting as a nucleophile, causing stability, increasing favourability, or increasing likelihood) within the base-free Wittig reaction. | “[Maleate's] ester groups stabilize the negative charge by resonance; [the deprotonation] is favourable, however unstable the carbanion is.” |
“This is because […] both esters in maleate are used to stabilize the ylide, [but the] acrylate does not have an additional [ester] group to provide resonance stabilization, so the base-free reaction could not proceed for acrylate.” |
Applying the codes required differentiating explicit properties from implicit properties in regard to the content, or the depth of students’ statements. Acidity, basicity, and stability are implicit properties, and we found that students could state them as implicit properties. For example, a student stated, “The acidity is due to the ester group drawing electron density away from the carbon […].” Here, acidity was based on the implicit property of induction effects. We read acidity as an implicit property; therefore, we coded the statement as an implicit-descriptive elaboration. However, we found that students could state them as explicit properties. For example, a student stated, “The [hydrogens] contained in the double bond [of malate] are much more acidic than [the hydrogens] contained in the double bond of acrylate.” Here, acidity was based on the explicit structure of hydrogen, neither based on the explicit property of charge nor based on the implicit properties of pKa, electronegativity, induction effects, or resonance effects. We read acidity as an explicit property; therefore, we coded the statement as an explicit-descriptive elaboration. We had to code the depth of students’ statements, although students could have had deeper knowledge and acidity is an implicit property.
Notably, Graulich et al. (2019) defined the codes by the depth of students’ statements, not by the correctness of them. Implicit statements do support correct statements (Graulich et al., 2019); however, an implicit statement can be either a correct or an incorrect statement, and it is not necessarily more correct than an explicit statement is. Therefore, through our analysis, we captured the elicitation of students’ elaborations in contrast to the correctness of their elaborations in order to assess our Wittig WTL assignment.
2. How do second-semester organic chemistry students’ elaborations change when the Wittig WTL assignment's rhetorical aspects change?
Year | Number of sections (N = 3) | Genre | Role | Audience |
---|---|---|---|---|
2018 | 1 | An essay for a laboratory course | A student in a laboratory course | An instructor of a laboratory course |
2 | A feature article for C&EN | A reporter for C&EN | Readers of C&EN | |
2019 | 3 | A cover letter for DowDuPont | An industrial chemist for DowDuPont | Management at DowDuPont |
2020 | 3 | A section of a grant proposal for NIH | A synthetic chemist in a medicinal research group | Reviewers at NIH |
2021 | 3 | A section of a grant proposal for NIH | A synthetic chemist in a medicinal research group | Reviewers at NIH |
A synthetic chemist in an ecological research group |
Specifically, a team of faculty (GVS), postdoctoral scholars (e.g., AJD), graduate research assistants (e.g., IZ), and undergraduate research assistants designed and redesigned five assignments. These changes were based on the Cognitive Process Theory of Writing (Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 1996), the WTL research (Gupte et al., 2021; Petterson et al., 2021), and reflections from research (i.e., the data analysis and our memos) throughout design and from practice (i.e., our conversations with students and UTAs and UTAs’ notes) throughout implementation. Yearly, we have considered how vocational interests motivate students (Stuckey et al., 2013) and how students’ motivations elicit their responses (Hayes, 1996). For example, in 2020, we discussed relevance, and we situated the assignment's rhetorical aspects in pre-health students’ vocational interests. Therefore, the 2020 Grant Proposal role was a synthetic chemist in a medicinal research group (Table 2). However, Petterson et al. (2021) reported that students felt that the assignment's rhetorical aspects, including the Wittig WTL assignment's, were too situated in pre-health students’ interests. In 2021, we discussed agency, and changed the assignment's rhetorical aspects. Therefore, the 2021 Grant Proposal role was a synthetic chemist in either a medicinal research group or an ecological group. This shifted the focus from instructor-assumed relevancy to student-determined relevancy. We recognize that the assignment's role is a small choice, but we believe that an assignment's design is a series of small choices with a big effect on teaching, learning, and, importantly and specifically, eliciting students’ responses.
We collected final drafts, and, hereafter, “final drafts” and “responses” are synonymous. Although we collected all responses, we found that a random subset of the responses reached saturation. We considered saturation to be when we could present representative, descriptive results but also when we could pull significant conclusions from those results. Therefore, we collected the 2018 Essay responses (n = 100; N = 471), the 2018 Feature Article responses (n = 100, N = 215), the 2019 Cover Letter responses (n = 100; N = 735), the 2020 Grant Proposal responses (n = 100; N = 714), and the 2021 Grant Proposal responses (n = 100; N = 788) via Canvas. We collected the 2018 surveys (N = 147), the 2019 surveys (N = 237), and the 2020 surveys (N = 83) via Qualtrics. No survey was administered in 2021.
Our analysis produced two sets of five distributions of four codes. To determine if the distributions of the codes changed when the assignments changed, we conducted a Pearson's chi-square test for homogeneity (McHugh, 2013) via StataSE 17.0. We infrequently applied the explicit-descriptive code, so we removed it, and we only evaluated the explicit-functional, implicit-descriptive, and implicit-functional codes. To determine which distributions of the codes changed, we separated the data by comparison (i.e., the comparison between Schemes 1 and 2 and the comparison between Schemes 2 and 3) and conducted Bonferroni pairwise comparisons for each pair of the assignments (Haynes, 2013). We used an adjusted alpha value (α = 0.005) to account for multiple comparisons (Haynes, 2013).
Theme | Definition | Examples |
---|---|---|
Rhetorical aspects misaligning with learning requirements | The assignment's rhetorical aspects misalign with its learning requirements, which hinders students’ depth of the elaboration. The students focus on writing the response rather than on explaining the assignment's learning objective. | “It was unclear – the [audience's] knowledge […] of organic chemistry. It was stated that they knew some but not all, and it was hard finding that in between point.” |
“I [found] it difficult to describe chemistry terminology in a way that is simple but also accurate for the target audience.” | ||
Rhetorical aspects aligning with learning requirements | The assignment's rhetorical aspects align with its learning requirements, which helps students’ depth of the elaboration. The students focus on explaining the assignment's learning objective rather than on writing the response. | “I liked that [the genre] challenged me to explain this to [the audience] very clearly, but without […] the technical words that I want to use.” |
“It was pretty cool writing a hypothetical grant proposal. […] I also liked that [the prompt's genre] allowed me to dig deeper about the implications of this reaction.” | ||
Rhetorical aspects engaging students | The assignment's rhetorical aspects engaged the students, which helps students’ depth of the elaboration. The students connect the assignment's learning objective to their laboratory course, their lecture course, or their out-of-class experiences. | “I liked how [the genre] allowed us to put the experiment that we used in the lab into a different situation and perspective.” |
“I liked how a lot of the information could be found by reading scientific journals, as [the role] made me practice analysing academic literature.” |
An example of these revisions is the 2020 Grant Proposal, which required two pages and recommended that students “write organized and logical paragraphs” and “use clear and concise language.” With this assignment, we noticed that the students’ responses were longer than the assignment's two-page requirement, and UTAs repeatedly reported that the students’ responses were redundant, neither “organized and logical” nor “clear and concise.” We interpreted that the assignment's learning objective (i.e., “Students will explain the mechanism's changes by connecting its explicit structures to their implicit properties”) and its questions were misaligned. Thus, students were writing more than they should, oftentimes writing what we wanted them to but sometimes not. In 2021, we pared and revised the questions, where we provided broad directions (e.g., “Compare the mechanistic steps in Scheme 2 to those in Scheme 1”), provided specific directions and questions (e.g., “What step allows for the formation of the ylide without the use of an external base?” and “Explain the structural features and electronic, chemical properties that are present or absent in this step”), and defined the vocabulary (e.g., “Structural features are atoms or functional groups”) (Appendix 1). Potentially, these transparent questions directed students’ responses from superfluous information to explicit and implicit elaborations.
Consequently, these revisions limit our claims. However, we revised how we asked the questions, not the focus of questions themselves or the assignment's learning objective. We found that students’ elaborations changed when the assignment's rhetorical aspects changed. Students’ surveys and prior research corroborated this influence (Gupte et al., 2021; Petterson et al., 2021). While we cannot claim that the rhetorical aspects were the sole influence, we interpret that the rhetorical aspects were a meaningful influence and that appropriate rhetorical aspects in combination with transparent questions are likely a more meaningful influence.
Additionally, we acknowledge that we implemented this assignment in a single course at a single institution. This implementation affects the assignment's transferability and, consequently, our claims’ generalizability. We encourage instructors and researchers to implement the 2021 Grant Proposal; however, we encourage them to reflect on the assignment's learning objective, their course's learning objectives, and their institution's context and, accordingly, to alter the assignment.
![]() | ||
Fig. 3 The distributions of elaborations for the comparison between Schemes 1 and 2 in Fig. 2. The Wittig WTL responses were coded as explicit descriptive (white bar), explicit functional (white dotted bar), implicit descriptive (black dotted bar), and implicit functional (black bar). |
As the assignments changed, explicit-functional elaborations decreased and implicit-functional elaborations increased by 2021 (Fig. 3). A Pearson's chi-square test (χ2(6) = 41.5; p = < 0.001) confirmed that the assignments had significantly different distributions, and Bonferroni pairwise comparisons (Table 4) found that the 2018 Essay and the 2018 Feature Article had significantly different distributions. Both assignments had more explicit elaborations than implicit elaborations. However, the 2019 Cover Letter, the 2020 Grant Proposal, and the 2021 Grant Proposal had more implicit elaborations than explicit elaborations. Therefore, a cover letter or a grant proposal met the assignment's learning objective more than an essay or a feature article did for the comparison between Schemes 1 and 2 (Fig. 2). Exemplifying this finding, we describe one of the first assignments (the 2018 Feature Article) and the final assignment (the 2021 Grant Proposal).
In one of the first assignments (the 2018 Feature Article), students’ elaborations were mainly explicit-functional elaborations (Fig. 3). Demonstrating explicit-functional elaborations, students stated explicit structures, such as the hydrogen atom, the carbon–oxygen double bond, or the carbon–carbon double bond. For example, a student connected having a hydrogen atom to being an acid, “This hydrogen is acidic due to the carbonyl group […].” Acidity was based on the hydrogen atom and the ester group, pulling on explicit structures. Also demonstrating explicit-functional elaborations, students stated explicit properties, such as the negatively-charged oxygen, the negatively-charged carbon, or the positively-charged phosphorus. For example, a different student connected having a negatively-charged carbon to being a base,
“The electrons on the O slide down, and the C–C double bond deprotonates the acidic H attached to the C with the P group […].
[Those] electrons fall into a C–P double bond […]. [In the] traditional mechanism, a separate base deprotonates […]. In this mechanism, the deprotonation occurs intramolecularly – hence why no base is needed.”
Basicity was based on the negatively-charged oxygen or the negatively-charged carbon, pulling on explicit properties. This focus on decoding explicit structures and explicit properties is a focus on explaining how the step happens, with explicit structures (e.g., “a base deprotonates”) and explicit properties (e.g., “the electrons slide down”) being actors in the step. Thus, these elaborations can be read as students practicing their mechanistic reasoning (Russ et al., 2008; Yan and Talanquer, 2015; Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Bodé et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood and Watts, 2022).
Contrastingly, in the last assignment (the 2021 Grant Proposal), students’ elaborations were mainly implicit-functional elaborations (Fig. 3). Demonstrating implicit-functional elaborations, students stated implicit properties, such as induction effects or resonance effects. For example, a student connected having induction effects to being an acid,
“The structural differences [between] the base-free Wittig reaction [and] the Wittig reaction are the carbonyl groups and the alkene. [These] double bonds provide electron shifting […]. […] Specifically, the hydrogen is acidic – or is easier to deprotonate – because the carbonyl groups withdraw electron density from the [neutral] carbon…”
Acidity was based not only on the hydrogen atom and the ester group but also on induction effects, pulling on an implicit property. The same student continued and connected having resonance effects to being a base,
“…and because the negatively-charged carbon [will be] resonance-stabilized by the carbonyl groups […]. [Resonance and induction] confer the deprotonation by the alkene and the elimination of a strong, external base.”
Basicity was based on not only the negatively-charged oxygen and the negatively-charged carbon but also on resonance effects, pulling on an implicit property. This focus on decoding implicit properties is a focus on explaining why the step happens, with implicit properties (e.g., “easier to deprotonate”) being descriptors of the step. Thus, these elaborations can be read as students practicing their causal reasoning (Russ et al., 2008; Yan and Talanquer, 2015; Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Bodé et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood and Watts, 2022).
Additionally, students compared a successful intramolecular Wittig reaction (Scheme 2) to an unsuccessful Wittig reaction (Scheme 3), in order to connect explicit structures to implicit properties (Fig. 2). This comparison was a univariate comparison, and the prime comparison was the acid, either the maleate with two ester groups or the acrylate with one ester group. Analysing students’ responses, we captured explicit-descriptive, explicit-functional, implicit-descriptive, and implicit-functional elaborations, and Fig. 4 presents these elaborations for this second comparison.
![]() | ||
Fig. 4 The distributions of elaborations for the comparison between Schemes 2 and 3 in Fig. 2. The Wittig WTL responses were coded as explicit descriptive (white bar), explicit functional (white dotted bar), implicit descriptive (black dotted bar), and implicit functional (black bar). |
As the assignments changed, implicit-functional elaborations stayed constant from 2018 to 2020, but they increased by 2021 (Fig. 4). A Pearson's chi-square test (χ2(6) = 31.5; p = <0.001) confirmed that the assignments had significantly different distributions, and Bonferroni pairwise comparisons (Table 5) found that the 2021 Grant Proposal had the significantly different distribution. The 2021 Grant Proposal had the most implicit elaborations, although all assignments had more implicit elaborations than explicit elaborations. Therefore, an essay, a feature article, a cover letter, or a grant proposal meets the assignment's objective for the comparison between Schemes 2 and 3 (Fig. 2). Exemplifying this finding, we describe the last assignment (the 2021 Grant Proposal).
In the last assignment (the 2021 Grant Proposal), students’ elaborations were mainly implicit-functional elaborations (Fig. 4). Demonstrating implicit-functional elaborations, students stated implicit properties, such as induction effects or resonance effects. For example, a student connected having induction effects to being an acid,
“[Scheme 3] lacks a second electron-withdrawing carbonyl group […]. […] The C–H bond in acrylate is stronger than the C–H bond in maleate [since] acrylate lacks the electron-withdrawing carbonyl that inductively weakens [maleate's] C–H bond. [This] makes [acrylate's] H less acidic, [and], if deprotonated, the structure would lack resonance stabilization. As this H cannot be intramolecularly deprotonated and no other bases [are] present, the acrylate cannot be turned into a ylide.” Acidity was based on: the ester group, starting with an explicit structure; induction effects, continuing with an implicit property; and bond strength, ending with another implicit property. Like the implicit-functional elaborations in the first comparison, these elaborations focus on decoding implicit properties (e.g., “is stronger”), focus on explaining why the step happens, and can be read as students practicing their causal reasoning (Russ et al., 2008; Yan and Talanquer, 2015; Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Bodé et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood and Watts, 2022). Unlike the implicit-functional elaborations in the first comparison, these elaborations start with an explicit structure, connect the explicit structure to an implicit property, and continue with a series of implicit properties (e.g., “acrylate lacks the electron-withdrawing carbonyl that inductively weakens [maleate's] C–H bond”) or “chain” (Graulich et al., 2019) and can be read as students practicing more chained, more elaborated causal reasoning.
Both comparisons are engaging students in decoding implicit properties; thus, both comparisons are engaging students in causal reasoning (Russ et al., 2008; Yan and Talanquer, 2015; Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Bodé et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood and Watts, 2022). However, the second comparison is engaging students in chaining implicit properties, or in more chained, more elaborated causal reasoning. This difference in the depth could be due to the difference in the comparison. The second comparison is a univariate comparison, where students decoded one explicit structure and constructed an elaboration from this explicit structure to multiple implicit properties. Contrastingly, the first comparison is a multivariate comparison, where students decoded multiple explicit structures and they constructed an elaboration from one of these explicit structures to one implicit property. Presumably, students weighed these explicit structures, after they decoded them and before they explained one of them. Accordingly, these elaborations are engaging students in multivariate reasoning (Kraft et al., 2010; Strickland et al., 2010; Sevian and Talanquer, 2014; Weinrich and Talanquer, 2016; Bodé et al., 2019; Moreira et al., 2019; Deng and Flynn, 2020), in addition to their causal reasoning (Russ et al., 2008; Yan and Talanquer, 2015; Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Bodé et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood and Watts, 2022). Engaging in both may be limiting the depth. Research has shown that a case comparison elicits multivariate reasoning (Caspari et al., 2018; Caspari and Graulich, 2019; Graulich and Caspari, 2020; Watts et al., 2021). Consequently, we assume that students’ multivariate reasoning is shaped by the question's case comparison, but we conjecture that their causal reasoning may be shaped by the assignment's rhetorical aspects.
In one of the first assignments (the Feature Article), students’ comments suggested that the rhetorical aspects and the learning requirements were misaligned. Defining this theme, the specified audience frustrated students. Specifically, a student felt frustrated interpreting C&EN readers’ knowledge, “It was unclear – the [audience's] knowledge […] of organic chemistry. It was stated that they knew some but not all, and it was hard finding that in between point.” This student interpreted that readers have some knowledge. Similarly, a student felt frustrated translating C&EN readers’ knowledge, “The translation of organic chemistry was unclear. How was I supposed to talk about [organic chemistry to] someone who doesn't know a lot about [organic chemistry]?” This student interpreted that C&EN readers do not have any knowledge. Students interpreted that C&EN readers are not a scientific audience, without any or with some knowledge, although we intended that they would be a general scientific audience.
Also defining this theme, the specified audience appeared to constrain students’ elaborations. Specifically, a student focused on formatting the response, “[The audience] was counterproductive and caused me to spend more time worrying about the formatting and writing style of the assignment instead of the content.” Similarly, a student focused on simplifying the response, “I [found] it difficult to describe chemistry terminology in a way that is simple but also accurate for the target audience.” Both students focused on writing the response, including style and length, rather than explaining the assignment's objective. Because students interpreted that C&EN readers are not a scientific audience, they assumed that vocabulary, phrases, and concepts were not shared and should be explained. However, the explanation must be “simplified” and explicit but also elaborated and implicit. Therefore, the audience poses an unsolvable rhetorical problem, choosing either fulfilling the assignment's rhetorical aspects or explaining the assignment's learning objective. This could account for the 2018 Feature Article not meeting the assignment's learning objective.
Contrastingly, in one of the final assignments (the Grant Proposal), students’ comments suggested that the rhetorical aspects and the learning requirements were aligned. Defining this theme, the audience satisfied students. For example, a student felt satisfied translating NIH readers’ knowledge, “I liked that [the genre] challenged me to explain this to [the audience] very clearly, but without […] the technical words that I want to use.” This student interpreted that reviewers have most knowledge. Students interpreted that NIH readers are a scientific audience, without all but with most knowledge, and we intended that they would be a specific scientific audience.
Also defining this theme, the specified audience appeared to encourage students’ elaborations. For example, a student focused on “working out” the reaction mechanisms, “[I]t is a fun challenge to write a highly technical essay. Working out the mechanism is very rewarding, as I feel like I have done something really difficult well.” This student focused on explaining the assignment's objective rather than on writing the response, including style and length. If students focused on writing the response, they focused on the length because they wanted to write more, not because they could not write more or did not know how much to write. For example, a student felt frustrated keeping to an assigned length, “A challenging part of the assignment was […] keeping my word [count] to a minimum.” Because students interpreted that NIH readers are a scientific audience, they assumed that most vocabulary, phrases, and concepts were shared and that the salient ones should be explained. Moreover, the explanation could be elaborated and implicit. Therefore, the audience poses a solvable rhetorical problem, balancing the assignment's rhetorical aspects and explaining the assignment's learning objective. This could account for the 2020 and 2021 Grant Proposals meeting the assignment's learning objective.
These findings show the influence of specifying an appropriate audience. We conjecture that students’ elaborations may be shaped by the assignment's audience, but we recognize that their elaborations may be truly shaped by their interpretations of the assignment's audience. Therefore, WTL assignments should not only specify an audience (Gere et al., 2019) but also detail an appropriate, transparent audience and describe the audience's level of knowledge or provide examples.
Additionally, we recognize that students’ elaborations may be shaped by their motivations. The Cognitive Process Theory of Writing (Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 1996) proposes that students not only write in response to the assignment's learning objectives (e.g., Should I include this concept?) and its rhetorical aspects (e.g., Does my audience know this concept? Should I explain this concept?) but also in response to their motivations (e.g., Do I want to include this concept? Do I want to explain this concept?). Students’ comments suggested that they were engaged. Defining this theme, students commented that the assignment “motivated [them],” the assignment “seemed meaningful,” or the assignment “seemed relevant.” Expanding on the assignment's relevance, a student connected the 2018 Feature Article to their laboratory course, “I like how [the assignment] incorporated what we were doing in the lab.” Similarly expanding on the assignment's relevance, a student connected the 2020 Grant Proposal to their lecture course, “I like that this assignment built upon [what] we had covered in lecture. The material was not totally unfamiliar, so I could actually attempt to explain it on my own.” Students found this assignment to be relevant, and studies corroborate that students find our assignments to be relevant (Petterson et al., 2021) and, because meaningful learning requires relevance (Bretz, 2001), that they find them to be meaningful learning experiences (Gupte et al., 2021). Relatedly, because motivation requires relevance (Stuckey et al., 2013), students might find our assignments to be motivating experiences. These findings inform ours, as students’ motivations have been proposed to mediate between an assignment's rhetorical problem (e.g., the audience) and its rhetorical solution (Hayes, 1996). Thus, we suspect that appropriate rhetorical aspects are those that motivate students in addition to – or in order to – encourage their elaborations.
The assignments elicited students’ explicit and implicit elaborations. Mechanistic reasoning depends on explicit elaborations, or decoding explicit structures (e.g., electrons, atoms, and chemical bonds); similarly, causal reasoning depends on implicit elaborations, or decoding implicit properties (e.g., acidity, induction effects, or resonance effects) (Russ et al., 2008; Yan and Talanquer, 2015; Cooper et al., 2016; Crandell et al., 2018; Bodé et al., 2019; Crandell et al., 2020; Dood and Watts, 2022). Thus, these elaborations can be interpreted as students practicing their reasoning and demonstrating their reasoning to researchers or instructors. The 2018 Essay and the 2018 Feature Article did not elicit these implicit elaborations to the extent that the 2019 Cover Letter, the 2020 Grant Proposal, and the 2021 Grant Proposal did, and, thus, the latter assignments better met the learning objective. Consistent with Watts et al.'s (2020) findings, our findings suggest that WTL assignments can support the learning objective of reasoning, in addition to the learning objectives of conceptual understanding that have been documented previously (Schmidt-Mccormack et al., 2019; Watts et al., 2020; Brandfonbrener et al., 2021; Finkenstaedt-Quinn et al., 2023).
Moreover, the assignments shaped students’ elaborations, moving from explicit elaborations to implicit elaborations. According to Flower and Hayes, the writing assignment is the “rhetorical problem,” and the writing response is the “rhetorical solution” (Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 1996) and an assignment's rhetorical aspects are one of the mediators between problem and solution (Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 1996). The 2018 Feature Article constrained students’ elaborations into explicit elaborations. Contextualizing this finding, students’ comments expressed that this assignment's audience presented a challenging and disengaging problem. Students interpreted that C&EN readers are not a scientific audience, although we intended that they would be a general scientific audience. They reported feeling frustrated, focusing on writing the response. Thus, the assignments’ rhetorical aspects and learning requirements were misaligned. Contrastingly, the 2020 and 2021 Grant Proposal assignments encouraged their elaborations into implicit elaborations. Contextualizing this finding, students' comments expressed that this assignment's audience presented a challenging but engaging problem. We intended that NIH reviewers would be a specific scientific audience, and students interpreted that they were. They reported feeling satisfied and capable, focusing on explaining the assignment's learning objective. Therefore, the assignments’ rhetorical aspects and learning requirements were aligned. These findings show how WTL assignments need to detail appropriate rhetorical aspects in order to encourage more implicit elaborations and, correspondingly, more complex reasoning.
Students’ motivations are also one of the mediators between problem and solution (Hayes and Flower, 1980; Flower and Hayes, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 1996). Students’ comments expressed that all assignments “motivated them,” “seemed relevant,” and “seemed meaningful.” Studies corroborate that students find WTL assignments to be relevant (Petterson et al., 2021) and to be meaningful learning experiences (Gupte et al., 2021). Supporting these findings, we report how WTL assignments need relevant rhetorical aspects in order to encourage more implicit elaborations and, correspondingly, more complex reasoning.
Benzoxepine analogs are important intermediates in the synthesis of therapeutic drugs. However, the isolation of benzoxepine analogs from natural sources is inefficient. Recently, German researchers synthesized benzoxepine analogs (Fig. 7) using a base-free Wittig reaction (Fig. 8). This reaction allows for the synthesis of therapeutic drugs on an industrial scale.
• write organized and logical paragraphs with headers;
• include figures that assist the reviewers in understanding complex information;
• use clear and concise language, striking a balance between organic jargon and oversimplifications.
1. What changes occur from the starting materials and the reagents to the products for the reaction in Scheme 2?
a. Describe the mechanistic steps.
b. Explain the structural features (e.g., atoms or functional groups) and the electronic, chemical properties (e.g., electronegativity, resonance, or induction) of the starting materials, reagents, and intermediates, and, moreover, explain their role (e.g., electrophile, nucleophile, acid, or base) or arrows in the mechanistic step.
c. Focus on the how and why as well as the what.
2. Compare the mechanistic steps in Scheme 2 to those in Scheme 1.
a. What step allows for the formation of the ylide without the use of an external base? (Note that the ylide is not shown in this scheme.)
b. Explain the structural features and electronic, chemical properties that are present or absent in this step.
3. Compare the mechanistic steps in Scheme 2 to those in Scheme 3.
a. Why does the reaction work with maleate but not work with acrylate?
b. Explain the structural features and electronic, chemical properties that are present or absent in this step.
You can and should include figures of schemes, structures, or mechanisms, if that supports your response. We suggest that you have the figure(s) in front of you — ready to color-code or mark-up in various ways — and that you use your visible thinking to guide your audience through your writing. Any images that you include in your response, including the figures in this prompt or those that you draw in ChemDraw or on paper, must have the original source cited using either ACS or APA format. Given your audience, your written response should suffice so that the written response is as representative and descriptive as the figures. You will be graded only on your written response.
• Read the response more slowly, keeping the rubric in mind.
• Highlight the pieces of text that let you directly address the rubric in your online responses.
• In your online responses, focus on higher-order concerns, like content and argument, rather than lower-order concerns, like grammar and spelling.
• Be very specific in your responses, referring to your peer's actual language, mentioning terms and concepts that are either present or missing, and following the directions in the rubric.
• Use respectful language, whether you are suggesting improvements to or praising your peer.
2. Does the author's comparison provide a description (the what) as well as an explanation (the how and why) of Scheme 1 occurring intermolecularly but Scheme 2 occurring intramolecularly? Comment on the structure and the electronic, chemical property that the author attributes as the reason.
3. Does the author's comparison provide a description as well as an explanation of Scheme 2 occurring but Scheme 3 not occurring? Comment on the structure and the electronic, chemical property that the author attributes as the reason.
Criteria | “Attempts” this criterion | Points (___/25) |
---|---|---|
Description and explanation of Scheme 2 | Includes: | 10 |
• A CORRECT description of the structural features OR what changes happen in the reaction mechanism AND | ||
• A CORRECT explanation of the electronic, chemical properties OR how and why changes happen in the reaction mechanism | ||
Comparison of Schemes 1 and 2 | Provides: | 3 |
• A structural feature as the comparison OR | ||
• A structural feature and its electronic, chemical property as the comparison | ||
Criterion (2) may be repetitive of Criterion (1), but Criterion (2) adds onto Criterion (1)'s explanation with a direct comparison between Schemes 1 and 2 | ||
Comparison of Schemes 2 and 3 | Includes: | 10 |
• A CORRECT description of the structural features OR what changes happen in the reaction mechanism AND | ||
• A CORRECT explanation of the electronic, chemical properties OR how and why changes happen in the reaction mechanism | ||
Appropriate language | Attempts: | 1 |
• Clear, concise sentences and logical, organized paragraphs | ||
DO NOT omit the point if they do not adhere to the grant proposal style | ||
Citations | Attempts: | 1 |
• Citations for figures they did not produce | ||
• Indications for figures they did produce (either ChemDraw or hand-drawn) |
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2024 |