Naomi Louise
Hennah
Northampton School for Boys, Science, Billing Road, Northampton, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland. E-mail: nhe@nsb.northants.sch.uk
First published on 31st December 2022
This case study demonstrates teaching and learning activities in the school laboratory, and employs talk moves for the direct assessment of practical task effectiveness. By adopting a sociocultural linguistic approach (SCLA), learning chemistry is understood to be a discursive process in which knowledge is constructed through social interaction and language. Thus, learning may be identified by attending to the language used in classroom discourse. The multimodal communication that took place during two acid and alkali practical lessons for learners aged 11 and 12 years was filmed and transcribed. Analysis of the transcripts revealed the language learning opportunities afforded by the tasks and demonstrated that school chemistry practical lessons can be understood in terms of three linguistic opportunities: introducing, using, and reflecting upon language. This lesson structure could be employed to plan more inclusive and equitable practical lessons which foreground language and value discussion equally to manipulating equipment. Recasting practical lessons as sites for learning and using the language of chemistry, key words introduced by the teacher are tracked and counted throughout the lesson to identify when they are used and by whom. The novel 3-part practical (3P) framework and multimodal discourse analysis are employed to assign the use of key words to the macroscopic, submicroscopic or symbolic level of thought. This analysis reveals the centrality of a results table to structuring talk and the detrimental effect of introducing novice learners to multiple levels of thought simultaneously. The Talk Identification (ID) Grid has been developed and used here to analyse student group discourses using talk moves to signpost learning in the domain of ideas and the domain of observables. Descriptors are provided to support instructors in identifying talk moves and how these moves relate to practical task effectiveness to target interventions that improve learning procedural and conceptual knowledge in the laboratory.
There is an acknowledged understanding in education that all students are language learners, and all teachers are language teachers (Bullock, 1975; de Oliveira, 2016) many teachers do not feel able to provide language support in their lessons beyond teaching scientific vocabulary (Markic, 2015; Quílez, 2021). However, vocabulary alone is not enough; instead students need to learn both words and how to use them (semantic structures) if they are to make the same meaning as their teachers (Lemke, 1990). Recommendations for teaching science to English language learners include: providing opportunities for productive discourse and interactions with others; using multiple modalities; and engaging in disciplinary practices (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2018). Such opportunities are provided by hands-on practical work when students are given the time to discuss their work, and to reflect upon their understanding of the pertinent scientific terms and concepts (Lemke, 1990; Tobin, 1990; Lunetta et al., 2007; Abrahams and Reiss, 2012; Gatsby, 2017). This can be considered through the lens of sociocultural linguistics.
Language and learning are inextricably linked (Halliday, 2004). Chemical concepts, as an example, do not exist in the abstract but are constructed by language blended with multimodal communication (O’Halloran, 2005, 2015; Gilbert, 2010), where multimodal is used here to refer to semiotic modes such as language, image, and gesture, rather than perceptual modes such as visual or haptic (Silliman et al., 2018).
Understanding the centrality of language in chemistry education substantiates the adoption of sociocultural linguistic approach and the application of language teaching and learning approaches in school chemistry. Understanding is challenged through exploratory talk (educationally effective talk) creating new knowledge, facilitating students to work in their zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 86). Student talk can be scaffolded using specific talk moves to develop discussion (Chin, 2006; Michaels and O’Connor, 2012). Scaffolds may be pre-planned macro-scaffolds or spontaneous micro-scaffolds (Nielsen and Hougaard, 2018). Talk moves are conceptualised as tools for facilitating academically productive talk (Michaels and O’Connor, 2015) and are used here to refer to talk occurring between teacher-student and student–student. Following from the work of Andersson and Enghag (2017), but in a school chemistry context, talk moves are conceptualised as tools that signpost how students talk during practical tasks. By identifying how students talk, targeted interventions may be implemented to better facilitate exploratory talk.
To be successful, learners of chemistry must be able to make connections among these varied representations (Yore and Treagust, 2006), but teachers should focus on one level of thought at a time to secure students’ understanding (Georgiadou and Tsaparlis, 2000; Tsaparlis et al., 2010).
Cognitive load theory explains that all tasks place a demand on the participant's working memory (the intrinsic load), and when instruction draws on multiple levels of thought, a high (extraneous) load is also placed on the learner's working memory. The greater the extraneous cognitive load imposed, the fewer the cognitive resources available for dealing with intrinsic cognitive load and so less learning occurs (Sweller et al., 2019).
The way information is presented during laboratory work is of particular importance, because the tasks themselves are demanding and impose a high cognitive load on the participants’ working memory (Johnstone and Wham, 1979).
The purpose of hands-on practical work may be understood to link the domain of ideas and the domain of observables (Tiberghien, 2000). The domain of observables is used here to refer to procedural knowledge: what is done with objects; and making observations. In contrast, the domain of ideas considers conceptual knowledge: the theories; and ideas that underlie the activity. Student talk during laboratory work has been reported to focus on the procedures needed to carry out the experiment (Russell and Weaver, 2011; Sandi-Urena et al., 2011), which suggests that learners are “manipulating equipment and not ideas” (Hofstein 2017, p. 366). The models presented by Tiberghien and Johnstone have been combined here (Fig. 1) to identify thinking by attending to talk that occurs during practical lessons.
![]() | ||
Fig. 1 Three levels of thought (after Johnstone, 1991, p. 78) aligned with the domain of ideas and the domain of observables (Tiberghien, 2000). |
Content and Language Integrated Learning is a language teaching approach that employs the target language for teaching and learning the subject matter (Dalton-Puffer et al., 2010; Dalton-Puffer, 2011). In Content and Language Integrated Learning science education, a practical lesson is understood to be composed of three distinct parts, each of which provides distinct linguistic opportunities (Nikula, 2015). Firstly, the pre-experimental phase exposes students to the subject's concepts, specialised vocabulary, and grammatical structures. Next, the experimental phase affords learners the opportunity to use the language modelled by the teacher during the introduction. Finally, the post-experimental phase can include metalinguistic work, thinking about the language used and how it is used. Nikula's description of a three-part practical lesson and Fig. 1 have been combined to develop the Three-Part Practical (3P) framework presented in Fig. 2.
The 3P framework operationalises key words (used here to refer to subject-specific language and symbolics) as markers that identify the domain and levels of thought being used during each part of the lesson. Multimodal data is required to contextualise the term, as for example, the word water could be used in talk concerning the procedure as a liquid at the macro level or in conceptual talk as a particle or molecule at the sub-micro level, and the meaning of H2O at the symbolic level. However, when the word is spoken by a person holding a measuring cylinder, the macro level is implied. The 3P framework will be used to evidence whether both domains are referred to, and whether different levels of thought are drawn upon.
The 3P framework provides a temporal view of language use during the lesson: key words introduced by the teacher can be tracked through the lesson to see who is saying what, and when. Understanding science requires more than knowing and using key words; learners must also use the same pattern of meaning relations (semantic structures) as their teacher to make the same meaning (Lemke, 1982). When a learner can use key words in the pattern that is valued by the scientific community, they demonstrate understanding.
The exploratory talk moves shown in Fig. 3, the Talk ID Grid, are operationalised here as tools, or signposts, for identifying conceptual and procedural learning during the practical task. It is intended that the Talk ID Grid could be used as an assessment for learning (Black and Wiliam, 1998) tool to support teachers in the direct assessment of the effectiveness of a practical task as a site for communicating chemistry. The Talk ID Grid characterises talk moves so that chemistry educators can target interventions that develop the quality of student talk and facilitate conceptual and procedural learning.
![]() | ||
Fig. 3 Talk ID Grid identifying exploratory talk moves in the domain of observables and in the domain of ideas at both level 1 linguistic, and level 2 cognitive to determine the effectiveness of a practical task for communicating (derived from Millar and Abrahams, 2009; Andersson and Enghag, 2017). |
1. What kinds of opportunities do practical investigations afford students for learning the language of chemistry?
2. To what extent can student talk be used to determine a practical task's effectiveness as a site for communicating chemistry?
The video recordings were taken by four cameras, one focused on the front of the classroom where the teacher habitually stands and demonstrates, two on benches positioned where the student groups carry out their practical tasks, and one accompanying the researcher as field notes were taken and students were interviewed. In total more than 10 hours of recordings were made. The video recordings from the two lessons were categorised by camera location and were transcribed verbatim. The data was cleaned and triangulated by the researcher by repeatedly watching the recordings and comparing the transcripts from different cameras to each other and to the field notes. The written work produced by each group was also collected and used to support and validate data from the recordings in the absence of a second researcher.
To preserve anonymity, students habitually seated on the bench with camera 1 are referred to as Group 1 collectively and S followed by a number (S1, S2, and S3) individually. Students habitually seated on the bench with camera 2 are referred to as Group 2 collectively and S followed by a letter (SG, SK, and SL) individually. The teacher is coded as T, and class members not in Groups 1 and 2 are assigned the generic code S.
The work was conducted in compliance with the British Education Research Association ethical guidelines (BERA, 2018) and British Association for Applied Linguistics (2021), aligning with the principles of informed consent, right to withdraw, and guarantee of anonymity. The school Headmaster acts as an overseer of all actions conducted in the school, and permission to complete this research was confirmed by him. All data collected and used during this research was securely stored and although permission was granted by participants and their caregivers, and all the images used have been treated to prevent the identification of participants.
An inductive thematic analysis as described by Ary et al. (2018) of the transcripts was conducted to expose language learning activities in each lesson part. Identification and coding of classroom strategies including initiation response feedback (IRF) questioning, were informed by Lemke's guide for recognising teacher and student strategies of control (1990, Appendix 2).
After identifying and classifying all the spoken episodes, the original recordings of sequences of interest were revisited. The transcription of these sequences was expanded to include a multimodal analysis of corresponding actions and images. The multimodal analysis was informed by Bezemer and Mavers (2011) and Flewitt (2011).
Word frequency analysis was used to understand students’ language use in each of the three parts of the practical lesson using the 3P framework. Microsoft Excel was used for the transcription of talk from both practical lessons as demonstrated by Bree and Gallagher (2016). The discourse was separated into a turn per row of the spreadsheet and the search function was used to locate words of interest in the data. The key words were identified inductively through the transcription process. Once the term was located, the speaker and context were noted, and the frequency of use was calculated and tabulated in the 3P framework.
To address the second RQ, the transcripts of student-student dialogue as they perform the Litmus and Neutralisation lessons were deductively coded into one of three different talk types, disputational, cumulative, and exploratory talk (Mercer, 1995). Individualised decision making, and disagreement characterise disputational talk, the exchanges are short and composed of: assertions; counter assertions; competing; and defending. Cumulative talk is characterised by: instruction; repetition; confirmation; elaboration; and although positive, it is uncritical, so ideas are not challenged nor justified. In contrast, exploratory talk is both positive and challenging: criticism is both constructive and justified; opinions are sought, and joint decisions are made; everyone actively participates; the exchanges are longer and demonstrate reasoning.
The Talk ID Grid was then applied to assess the practical tasks’ effectiveness as a site for communicating the language of chemistry. A task that facilitates exploratory talk is more effective than one that does not, however, the extent of a practical task's effectiveness (Abrahams and Millar, 2008; Millar and Abrahams, 2009) may also be determined using the talk moves associated with conceptual and procedural talk.
Litmus lesson pre-experimental phase (from Extract 1)
Teacher: what is it? [row 7]
Student: is it, eh, is it an acid waffle or something like that? [row 8]
Teacher: Shush, good idea but no. [row 10]
Student: Pallet [row 13]
The students suggest names consistent with similar looking more familiar items like “a pallet”. Through repeating the IRF sequence the teacher provides the students with time and opportunity to consider the apparatus directing his body and gaze to the student answering, and by doing so demonstrates that he values their suggestions. Barnes (2010), describes this process as “active learning” whereby ideas are shared and shaped between interlocutors, forging links between new and existing knowledge.
Extract 2 from the Neutralisation task also demonstrates language instruction, the students are exposed to key words (acid, alkali, neutral), both through the teacher's talk and a written procedure that is projected onto the board. Furthermore, the teacher's explanation of neutralisation has drawn upon the semiotics of chemistry (H+, OH−) in both the visible and auditory modes.
In the excerpt below, the teacher is standing next to the particle representations drawn on the board and is holding the conical flask containing the green neutral solution as he speaks.
Neutralisation lesson pre-experimental phase (from Extract 2)
Teacher: 23 drops boys, at this point it's neutral. [row 125]
Teacher: All the OH's have combined with the H's and made water, so in there now is just water. [row 126]
Teacher: Not acid not alkali because all the OHs and Hs have joined to make water. [row 127]
The teacher physically and verbally links the domains of observables and ideas, a “contextualization of concepts” (Jiménez-Aleixandre and Reigosa, 2006, p. 708). In doing the teacher is simultaneously drawing on all three levels of thought (Johnstone, 1991).
The extract begins when Student 3 has just arrived and joins Group 1 as they begin testing their fourth solution, deionised water. In this excerpt, Student 2 directs Student 3 to test the solution first with red then blue litmus paper following the column sequence displayed in the results table.
Litmus lesson experimental phase (from Extract 3)
Student 2: litmus, red then blue. [row 136]
Student 3: Nothing happened, it's the same. [row 144]
Student 1: No change, it is not an alkali. [row 145]
When Student 3 reports the red litmus paper result to the group, Student 1 nods and then rephrases the observation in the manner previously modelled by the teacher. The student's talk and actions are directed by the results table, and they can express their observation using the teacher's language pattern.
The experimental phase of the Neutralisation lesson is noisy and unsettled. Extract 4 begins when Group 2 are adding sodium hydroxide solution dropwise to a conical flask containing hydrochloric acid and universal indicator. The student's need to count and record the number of drops added and the concomitant colour change.
Neutralisation lesson experimental phase (from Extract 4)
Student L: 20 [row 93]
Student K: 30 [row 94]
Student L: Oh yeah, write 30 turned orange. [row 95]
The students’ language is indexical and dependent on the details of the practical task with very little use of subject-specific language and no consideration of what the results may mean. There appears to be a lack of collaboration as Student L dominates both the talk and equipment. These students do not appear to have adopted the language modelled by their teacher when introducing the neutralisation task.
Litmus lesson post-experimental phase (from Extract 5)
Student K: In red litmus no change. [row 92]
Teacher: So, what does that tell us? [row 93]
Student K: That there wasn’t, that it's not an [ac] alkali. [row 94]
Teacher: Good, it wasn’t an alkali. [row 95]
Student K: Observation with blue litmus, it turned red so,
it was an acid. [row 96]
The teacher has reinforced the requirement to test the solution with both red and blue litmus papers then record the results and analysis, by using an IRF sequence to model the language and thought pattern required to do so. The teacher gives verbal affirmation and repeats the student's analysis whilst recording the result in the table on the board. The feedback in this IRF sequence can be understood as a scaffold, specifically a spontaneous micro-scaffold, as the student is able to respond fluently with the next result and analysis.
In Extract 6 from the Neutralisation lesson, the teacher draws upon a range of modes to regulate difficulty and negotiate meaning during an IRF sequence.
Neutralisation lesson post-experimental phase (from Extract 6)
Teacher: If we’re weakly acidic what are we going to have more of? [row 14]
Student: H plus. [row 15]
Teacher: Yeah, we’re still going to have more H plus aren’t we. [row 16]
Teacher: I’m going to put extra H plus, just to mean that there is not as many as the excess H plus in the strong acid. [row 17]
The teacher uses gesture to draw the student's attention toward the particle diagram on the board to scaffold his question and the student successfully negotiates the teacher's meaning. Having elicited the student's response, the teacher then marks the importance of the point by confirming and then rephrasing the answer (Lemke, 1990).
Furthermore, the teacher critically reflects on language and engages in metalinguistic work by writing and talking about his choice of terms “extra” and “excess” to denote a change in magnitude.
In summary, although the Litmus and Neutralisation lessons were not planned as language lessons, there is evidence that the teacher and the students are involved with language teaching and learning activities consistent with the Content and Language Integrated Learning science three-part practical lesson model (Nikula, 2015).
![]() | ||
Fig. 4 Litmus Task 3P Framework of key word frequency use during each phase of the lesson demonstrates that the students are working in the domains of observables and ideas. |
![]() | ||
Fig. 5 Neutralisation Task 3P Framework of key word frequency use during each phase of the lesson demonstrates that the students are not working in the domain of ideas. |
The frequency of key word use shown in Fig. 4 and 5 indicate that teacher talk dominates the pre-and post-experimental phases of the lessons. As previously demonstrated in the pre- and post-experimental phase Litmus and Neutralisation lesson exerts above, the teacher controlled the talk by selecting respondents one at a time, whereas during the experimental phase up to six students were talking, thus the word frequency values increased. The use of the word acid for example, is used by Student 1 in the Litmus lesson experimental phase (Extract 3) and by two other students in the Litmus lesson post-experimental phase (Extract 5). However, it is not always possible to assign the use of a word to a particular student during group interaction.
The Litmus lesson data presented in Fig. 4 shows an increase in frequency of students using key words from the pre-experimental phase to the post-experimental phase which may indicate that the practical task has increased students’ familiarity and confidence in using the terms as learning to apply words correctly facilitates understanding their meaning (Toulmin, 1972).
The Litmus lesson (Fig. 4) demonstrates 78 incidences of key terms being used by the students during the experimental phase but only 25 are recorded during the Neutralisation lesson (Fig. 5). These results indicate that the Neutralisation task is less effective than the Litmus task at facilitating students to adopt the language modelled by their teacher during the pre-experimental phase. For example, Fig. 5 records one incidence of a student using the word neutral, which in occurred during the Neutralisation lesson pre-experimental phase (Extract 2, row 124).
Comparing Fig. 4 and 5 demonstrates that student talk in the domain of ideas occurs more often during the Litmus lesson (values shown in bold) specifically, there are 26 occurrences compared to zero during the Neutralisation lesson.
In Fig. 5, the teacher is recorded using symbolics during both the pre- and post-experimental phases of the Neutralisation lesson, whereas there is no evidence of students doing so during the experimental phase. In the Neutralisation lesson post-experimental phase excerpt (from Extract 6), the teacher provided a micro-scaffold for Student 1 to respond, “H plus”. This is likely to be indexical as there is no evidence to suggest that hydrogen ions nor the submicroscopic Arrhenius model of acids is understood.
Firstly, in the post-experimental phase excerpt (from Extract 6), the teacher repeatedly refers to “H plus”, as an oral abbreviation of hydrogen ions. In doing so, the teacher is implicitly moving between the submicroscopic and symbolic levels of thought. Instruction that moves between multiple levels of thought increases the extraneous cognitive load placed on the learner's working memory to the detriment of learning (Milenković et al., 2014).
Secondly, colour is a semiotic mode used to convey meaning (Pantaleo, 2012) the particle models drawn on the board (reproduced in Extracts 2 and 6) show the hydrogen ion in blue and hydroxide ion in blue and red. An expert will recognise that the hydrogen nucleus is common to both ions and be able to decode the teacher's meaning, but it may be distracting for a novice and add to the extrinsic cognitive load Miller et al. (2019).
Finally, in the pre-experimental phase excerpt (from Extract 2) the teacher states, “All the OHs have combined with all the Hs and made water…” and “Not acid, not alkali because all the OHs and Hs have joined to make water” in which the ellipsis of H+ to H and OH− to OH has occurred. Ellipsis is defined as the deletion of linguistic elements that can be understood from contextual clues (Bussmann et al., 2006), here; the contextual clues are derived from the multimodal data available to the students. However, the resultant terms are incorrect and the unconscientious modelling of imprecise language compounds the difficulty in learning chemistry. If a learner's understanding of scientific language can be facilitated by combining the appropriate everyday language with scientific language (Rees et al., 2021) then resorting to the use of symbolics as an oral shorthand may be avoided.
The teacher's use of problematic language during the neutralisation lesson can be related to the commercially available course and assessment materials used by the school. These materials require 11 and 12 year-olds to represent neutralisation as the symbol equation for the formation of water from hydroxide ions and hydrogen ions (Gardom Hulme et al., 2013). This requirement disrupts curriculum coherence (Gardner et al. 2014) as the particle model and atoms are new ideas for the young learners, whereas ions and the Arrhenius model of acids are met in the 14–16 year-old's curriculum (UK Government, 2014). The ellipsis may have arisen because the teacher is avoiding introducing and discussing ions and charges, thus symbols and part-symbols are used without acknowledging the Arrhenius definitions underlying submicroscopic ideas. The ellipsis and the use of verbal shorthand indicate that both learning and teaching difficulties arise when knowledge is presented in steps (Danili and Reid, 2004) that disrupt the hierarchical sequence of scientific ideas (McPhail, 2021).
• There were fewer incidences of learners using the language introduced by the teacher during the Neutralisation lesson
• None of the language associated with the domain of ideas was used by students in the experimental phase of the Neutralisation lesson
• Only the Neutralisation lesson used chemical symbols and drew on all three levels of thought
The analysis of the students’ language use during the Litmus lesson indicates that the learners have assimilated the language modelled by the teacher as both the key words and the pattern in which they were used were evident in the experimental phase.
RQ 2: To what extent can student talk be used to determine a practical task's effectiveness as a site for communicating chemistry?
The experimental phase for each lesson lasted approximately 40 minutes. Transcripts of student talk during the hands-on practical tasks were deductively coded into sequences corresponding to Mercer's (1995) typology of talk. Most of the student–student talk coded as cumulative; an example of which is provided in the excerpt below.
Litmus lesson experimental phase cumulative talk (from Extract 7)
Student 1: I’m doing the next one [row 44]
Student 2: You need to write it turns red [row 45]
Student 1: I have [row 46]
Student 1: No, I haven’t [row 47]
Student 2: You get the water thing and I’ll get litmus [row 48]
The student interaction is positive but there is a lack of discussion which results in a series of parallel statements rather than dialogue. Extract 7 and the cumulative talk moves described by the Talk ID Grid are available in Appendix 4.
There was only one instance of disputational talk identified from the transcripts, an example of which is provided in the excerpt below.
Litmus lesson experimental phase disputational talk (from Extract 8)
Student G Stop! [row 80]
Student G Write it down first [row 81]
Student L So blue no change [row 82]
Student G What you’ve already done it? [row 83]
Student G When we weren’t looking? [row 84]
The excerpt indicates competition within the group and individualised decision making rather than consensus. Extract 8 and the disputational talk moves described by the Talk ID Grid are available in Appendix 5.
One talk sequence from Group 1 during the Litmus task (Extract 3) was coded as exploratory talk as: the students were constructively critical of each other's ideas; and worked together to collect and analyse their data. This exploratory talk sequence was then deductively coded into the four quadrates of the Talk ID Grid which has been divided for clarity into Fig. 5, the domain of observables and Fig. 6, domain of ideas.
The level 2 exploratory talk action moves in the domain of observables reveal that Student 3 is both carrying out the practical task, and is actively considering the result's meaning by asking “So if they both don’t change then it has to be neutral?” Student 1 affirms and shares the data in his table of results to justify his response. These action moves indicate that the group are constructing knowledge and understanding of their actions in a process of active learning that may facilitate recall of the activity later.
The Level 2 purpose moves in the domain of ideas (Fig. 7) demonstrate how common knowledge has been constructed through the negotiation of the meaning of the experiment results. The exchange culminates when Student 3 states “Ok it's water, that makes sense.” aligning this new knowledge with his prior knowledge by recognising that deionised water, like tap water, must be neutral. Thus, Student 3 conveys his thinking through talk, and demonstrates that the analysis of the result has been internalised as, learning chemistry requires integration of the scientific viewpoint with existing ideas (Scott et al., 2011) suggesting that he will still be able to demonstrate this understanding later.
Group 1 also used cumulative talk during the Litmus task, and it was the late arrival of Student 3 that triggered the change to exploratory talk. In Extract 3, Student 3 is showing interest and engagement with the Litmus task, but he is also accepted and supported by Student 1 and Student 2 and; it is this combination that facilitates collaboration and exploratory talk.
Fig. 7 the domain of ideas, documents Student 3 looking at his partner's results and asking, “So if they both don’t change it has to be neutral?” This initiates an extended talk sequence where the more knowledgeable partners help Student 3 construct new knowledge. From this perspective, the results table is performing a new role beyond dictating what is recorded to orchestrating dialogue.
The 3P framework could be used to plan practical lessons in which: key words and language patterns are foregrounded; and problematic language identified. Further, the lesson structure affords time for: student discussion during the practical task; and for discussion between the teacher and students when the hands-on activity is completed.
Patterns of language used by both the teacher and the students, replicated the structure of the results table. Understanding that a table of results may impact student talk, affords educators with the opportunity to decide in advance what they want the students to talk about and design the table accordingly. In this way, the teacher may regard the results table as a macro-scaffold for student talk (Nielsen and Hougaard, 2018).
The analysis of a sequence of exploratory talk using the Talk ID Grid, demonstrated the talk moves associated with an effective practical task. The Talk ID Grid operationalises talk moves as signposts of conceptual and procedural learning during practical work. This could help teachers to identify learning from student–student talk and to intervene when required with spontaneous micro-scaffolds such as repeating, revoicing, or questioning (Chin, 2006; Michaels and O’Connor, 2012, 2015; Nielsen and Hougaard, 2018).
Direct assessment is part of most teaching episodes: scanning the room to see if the task is complete; checking laboratory equipment is being used safely; listening to tones of voice; and noting body language in case someone needs help. Similarly, student talk moves described in the Talk ID Grid (available in the Appendix 1) could be used to indicate that a group needs help managing collaboration or discussion. As teachers become more familiar with the range of talk moves identified in the Talk ID Grid, the moves could be employed as signposts for assessment for learning (Black and Wiliam, 1998). For example, the Talk ID Grid could be used to classify group talk during a practical task to identify aspects of the intended learning that need to be reinforced in subsequent lessons. In addition, targeted modifications to the practical task could be devised to make future iterations more effective as a site for communicating chemistry.
![]() | ||
Fig. 8 An example of a possible Neutralisation task results table designed to be a student talk macro-scaffold. |
The discursive and action moves associated with the domain of observables shown in Fig. 6, relate to the ways the students are interacting whilst carrying out the practical task. During this episode of exploratory talk, the students were working collaboratively (Kirschner et al., 2009) to create common knowledge. Adopting protocols such as Lab Roles and Lab Talk, as described in our earlier work (Hennah et al., 2022), could facilitate more episodes of collaboration and exploratory talk during future iterations.
Research has shown that formal and explicit instruction in collaborative skills is requisite for classroom collaboration to occur (Le et al., 2018). The co-occurrence of collaboration and exploratory talk observed in this case study is of interest, as approaches known to facilitate collaboration may also stimulate exploratory talk.
The study and the frameworks presented were designed by a teacher who, motivated by an apparent language barrier in teaching and learning chemistry, sought to understand language, and learning in practical lessons. In doing so, future interventions could be targeted to better support learning in the context of the school chemistry laboratory observed, however, the study may also be useful to other educators, and researchers seeking to foster the quality of student talk and collaboration.
The Talk ID Grid to for the domain of ideas.
Litmus task method
1. Tear each piece of litmus paper into three smaller pieces.
2. Place a small piece of red litmus paper into one well of the spotting tile.
3. Using a pipette, add a drop of sulphuric acid to the red litmus paper.
4. Record your observation in the results table (shown below).
5. Repeat steps 1 to 4 with a small piece of blue litmus paper.
6. Using the two litmus paper results complete the analysis column of the table.
7. Repeat steps 1 to 6 with the remaining five solutions.
8. Dispose of the pieces of litmus paper in the waste bin.
Neutralisation lesson Learning Objective: Describe how pH changes in neutralisation reactions and relate these changes to the colour of universal indicator.
Neutralisation task method
1. Pour 10 cm3 hydrochloric acid into a conical flask.
2. Add a few drops of universal indicator and swirl the beaker carefully.
3. Pour 9 cm3 sodium hydroxide solution into a second beaker.
4. Using a pipette, carefully add the sodium hydroxide drop by drop to the conical flask containing acid and universal indicator. Keep swirling.
5. As the sodium hydroxide is added note the colour changes and the number of drops added to produce the change.
Cumulative talk moves in the domain of ideas in which: content moves characterise students’ analysis of data; and purpose moves which describe how the students think together to understand their data.
Disputational talk moves in the domain of ideas in which: content moves characterise students’ analysis of data; and purpose moves which describe how the students think together to understand their data.
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2023 |