Karina
Adbo
* and
Clara
Vidal Carulla
Department of Biology and Environmental Science, Linnaeus University, SE-39182 Kalmar, Sweden. E-mail: clara.vidalcarulla@lnu.se
First published on 17th April 2019
This study focuses on the design of play-based learning activities for chemistry in preschool. Viewing chemistry as a part of our past and present culture instead of as a subject, provides the backdrop for a more holistic approach to chemistry within this specific environment. A cultural-historical perspective, together with scaffolding, emergent science skills and sustained shared thinking, made up the framework for the design of the learning activities. Results show that when scaffolding and emergent science skills are used within the design, they provide good support for both the content and the teacher in the actual learning situation. Working with scaffolding was also beneficial for professional development. However, for a progressive and inclusive activity design, it is essential to take into account aspects of the immediate environment and methods for direct evaluation.
For this purpose, deriving subject-specific content for this educational level becomes a point of interest. This investigation was designed as a longitudinal study spanning over three years with the overall purpose to better understand children's emergent science and concept development when chemistry is introduced into a preschool setting. To achieve this purpose in a respectful manner, given the age of the participants (three years old when the study was launched), it was essential at the outset to derive chemical content relevant for preschool, while making sure that concepts remained connected within their framework. This first part of the study reported here is focused on the design and content of play-based learning activities. In order to design activities with chemistry content within this particular culture, where play, social interactions, history, affective connections and imagination make up the basic setting, the study places itself within the cultural-historical perspective (Fleer and Pramling, 2015).
Indeed, early childhood science education can, in principle, be separated into different teacher-led efforts, namely, discovery learning and science as a cultural process. In the case of discovery learning (Fleer and Pramling, 2015), learning is considered to be a child-centred approach where the teacher's choice of activities stems from interpretations of the child's own questions and ideas. Critique raised against this approach is that interpretations are difficult and much is lost in the translation of the child's actions by the teacher (Fleer and Pramling, 2015). Another concern is that the teacher's use of this approach sometimes results in the child being left to her own exploration and not provided with active teacher support to further her experiences.
However, when learning is seen as a cultural process (Vygotsky, 2016), the teacher's choice of activities cannot be separated from learning situations. Here, learning is seen as achieved and motivated through the child's social interactions with the external world, as opposed to originating from the individual exploring his or her surroundings (as in the case of discovery learning). An important distinction between these two paradigms is that in the case of the cultural process, preschool is more teaching-centred (Fleer, 2010; Cutter-Mackenzie and Edwards, 2013; Siraj-Blatchford, 2009).
Teaching efforts within this educational level are mainly focused on helping children develop their everyday concepts, as they are seen as the starting point for further development. Everyday concepts have been defined by Fleer (2010) as “concepts imbedded in everyday situations, which support the child to undertake everyday things” (p. 48). Using everyday concepts as a starting point for development also calls for a differentiation between what is perceived as a word and what is perceived as a concept.
Within this study the differentiation made by Eshach and Fried (2005) is used. They argue that the distinction between a word and a concept is that a concept holds numerous different types of information, all of which contribute to a richer and more diverse understanding of the concept at hand. The authors illustrate this using “cat”, where “knowing the word cat and knowing the concept of cat are two different achievements … The concept, cat does not only consist of verbal information such as a cat is an animal with four legs, fur, etc., but also visual information … haptic information … aural information … olfactory information” (Eshach and Fried, 2005, p. 10). That is to say, the concept can also contain an image of a cat, the memory of the touch of a cat, the memory of the relative weight and claws of a cat, together with the sound of a cat and maybe even the smell of a cat. This differentiation between concepts and words is made in the present study.
In preschool environments, emergent science is becoming increasingly important as it is recognised that science learning “cannot take place without the establishment of a long-term relationship between the world of science and the child” (Fleer, 2009a). Expanding children's vocabularies and developing their everyday concepts provides new ways for children to describe and understand their surrounding world, which is here seen as contributing to both imagination and creativity. If children have rich everyday experiences of the environment, they have more possibilities for imaginative and creative thought and action, that in turn provide them with more perspectives to use when viewing the world. As Vygotsky (2004) suggested, “imagination is based on our experiences while creativity is a function of our ability to combine experiences” (p. 9). A long-term relationship between the child and science is also beneficial since it helps in developing attitudes towards science (Fleer and Pramling, 2015, p. 64). Motives is the word used for describing this attitude when science becomes a natural part of a child's way of acting or thinking. Developing motives for science can be achieved when children are “actively encouraged to take part in science activities” (Fleer and Pramling, 2015, p. 31). Wonder, on the other hand, holds a personal and emotional component that together with experience “acts as a prism through which the world is experienced by the child” (Fleer and Pramling, 2015, p. 64). This emotional aspect is also of essence since it provides motivation and, therefore, improves learning outcomes (Cherniss et al., 2006) as well as supports the child's developing imagination (Hedegaard and Fleer, 2013). Fleer (2010) summarised the cultural-historical perspective as, “it seeks to examine scientific learning in relation to how everyday situations create scientific encounters that are emotionally charged and socially mediated in actions and activities”.
Other considerations that need to be made when designing an activity are “contextual intersubjectivity” and “conceptual intersubjectivity” (Fleer, 2010). This implies that it is not only vital to consider how material is introduced to the child, but also how the conceptual content fits into its own context as part of a knowledge system. “What is important for the success of a cultural-historical approach … is for the teacher to determine core concepts” (Fleer, 2010, p. 94), to connect concepts into their theoretical context. Studies have shown that preschool teachers in general are very good at finding science content in the everyday context of the preschool, but that the analysis made by the teacher often remains just an analysis that is difficult to transform into practice (Fleer, 2010).
Subcategories | Descriptions of subcategories | |
---|---|---|
(a) Task recruitment | ||
1 | Choosing a relevant topic | Draw on children's interest |
2 | Referring to children's own interests | Using topics raised by children, or what do you know about…? Or what would you like to know about…? |
3 | Referring to children's emotions | I want you to feel… See if you like… |
(b) Reinforcing children's self esteem | ||
4 | Presenting children as experts | Telling the child that he/she is an expert |
5 | Commending children | That is very interesting… |
6 | Announcing children's names | Noticing the individual |
7 | Avoiding judgement | Do not reject ideas or answers even if they are wrong/irrelevant |
Subcategories | Descriptions of subcategories | |
---|---|---|
(a) Clarification and goal orientation | ||
1 | Verbalising | Providing verbal explanations of the task |
2 | Repeating task description | Using same or different wording |
3 | Modelling | Showing how to do the task |
4 | Sharpening the focus of observation | Explaining what children should look at and focus on in the observation |
5 | Using similarities | Describing object/phenomenon by relating to another object and phenomenon having similar properties |
6 | Repeating children's answers | Repeating children's answers in the same or different wording so others understand |
(b) Task reduction | ||
7 | Voicing the beginning of terms | Saying out loud the beginning of new terms |
8 | Providing parts of the answer | And letting the children finish the sentence |
9 | Purposely providing the wrong but close answer | Asking questions as statements…for example, placing one object on one side of a scale and when about to place an equal object on the other side of the scale, ask if the other side of the scale will be lower now |
10 | Providing gesture hints | As in the above example, teacher uses her hands to illustrate the two sides of the scale |
11 | Suggesting that children help one another | |
(c) Diagnosis and calibration | ||
12 | Checking previous knowledge | Asking questions that require knowledge gained from previous experience. Usually these are what/why questions |
13 | Asking for clarification | Asking children to explain the meaning of what they said |
14 | Changing task | In cases that children show difficulty dealing with a question/task, the teacher changes the wording or the question/task |
(d) Encourage higher-order thinking language | ||
15 | Encouraging children's questioning | Directly asking children to pose questions |
16 | Avoiding answering/judging | Same as for the affective domain |
17 | Using thinking language | Using terms such as hypothesis, investigation, experiment, observation and so on |
18 | Asking hypothesis questions | If we put more (concentrated) liquid in, what will happen to the colour? |
19 | Asking questions requiring connecting between variables | Asking questions where the teacher refers to one variable and asks what will happen to the other variable? |
20 | Reflecting and metacognitive tasks | Asking children questions regarding the process they went through in gaining knowledge about a new topic they learned |
(e) Withdrawal techniques | ||
21 | Using same/similar language in different activities | |
22 | Using same/similar sequence of strategies | The scientific method “stop and think” what do we already know? |
The method is here seen as highly suitable for the preschool level and for the design of conceptual play learning activities as it includes both cognitive considerations, such as the reduction of variables to increase focus on the task at hand, and emotional variables that ensure contextual inter-subjectivity. To meet the demands for conceptual inter-subjectivity and to determine the important concepts in order to “give directions for pedagogical framing in concept formation” (Fleer, 2010, p. 93) the core concepts and the “culture” of chemistry need to be taken into consideration.
Observation | Making use of all senses (including bodily memory and balance) |
The identification of similarities and dissimilarities | |
Finding connections in and between everyday objects and Science | |
Interpreting observations | |
Classification | Categorising items using one criterion |
Re-categorising items using more than one criterion | |
Categorising items using several criteria at the same time | |
Prediction | Predicting events |
Constructing evidence-based predictions | |
Realising that previous erroneous predictions can support future accurate predictions | |
Analysis | Verbalising events experienced in experiments and explorations |
Using/developing scientific language in explanations | |
Using everyday and scientific language for explaining observed events |
Focus | Chemistry skills |
---|---|
Material | Recognising that different materials are made of smaller particles. |
Using the words small and big or gestures to make the differentiation. | |
Filtering | Using the word filtering |
Recognising that there are particles of different sizes | |
Recognising what a filter does, recognising that there are many different filter sizes | |
Being able to select filter size for the task at hand | |
Recognising that there are particles that are so small that they cannot be filtered (through filters of everyday use) | |
Recognising that there are particles that are so small that they are invisible, finding different types of filters in the immediate environment | |
Mortaring | Using the word mortar |
Using the mortar in the intended way | |
Realising that matter can be divided | |
Finding different items that can be used for dividing matter | |
Realising the effect of mortaring on solubility or extraction | |
Recognising the use of mortaring in everyday situations | |
Dissolving | Using the word dissolving |
Recognising that there are differences in solubility between substances | |
Recognising that substances do not disappear when dissolved | |
Realising that substances are spread in all of the liquid | |
Recognising that substances spread in all of the liquid even if we do not stir | |
Realising that particles have movement | |
Stirring | Using the word stirring |
Recognising that stirring increases the speed of dissolving | |
Evaporation | Recognising that liquid turns to (air) gas |
Recognising that different substances turn to gas at different temperatures | |
Recognising that there are different substances dissolved in the liquid even if we don’t see them |
Aside from the above methods, the emergent science skills as defined by Johnston (2014) were intended to contribute to the pedagogical framing. For example, observation skills such as, “using all senses” also contributed to the differentiation of substances through taste, smell, texture and visual descriptions, including optical aids such as magnifying glasses. A different approach for deciding on chemical content has been suggested by Areljung (2017), such as design by making use of verbs, for example, mixing, stirring, separating, absorbing, gluing, sticking, dying, melting, freezing, evaporating and condensing.
The purpose of this study was to:
– design play-based learning activities with theoretical chemistry content and chemistry skills for the preschool environment.
– evaluate activities using the quality markers for play-based learning.
The preschool had a back garden with a play area, a workshop for outdoor toys and a carpentry shop with tools and paint brushes. The indoor environment included what can be seen as four different rooms, divided into different areas. One room had a sofa for relaxing and reading books, and a play-area with clothes for dressing up, as well as toys and construction items. There was another large room for more physical games. The third room was divided into one secluded area for arts and crafts, and one more open area that could serve as an extension to the arts and crafts area, but was also used for serving meals. Since the preschool had a cook, there was also a kitchen directly connected to the arts and crafts room.
The town where the preschool was located was semirural, and the children participating in the study were all native Swedish speakers. For the initial testing of the instrument, a group of four three-year-olds (n = 4) agreed to participate (two boys and two girls). Ethical approval for participating in the study, terminating participation, making videorecordings and publishing findings was obtained from the national ethical board, dean, staff and parents. To ensure approval from the children, ordinary staff, who knew the children well, were present at all times. Care was taken to ensure the anonymity of the preschool as well as the children. Data was collected during a four-week period twice a week, on Tuesdays and Thursdays, and the average activity was 20–30 minutes long.
Preschools are here viewed as micro-cultures, all unique due to context, resources, focus, staff, parents and children. As the knowledge that comes to the fore in all preschool activities is dependent on all active parties, data collection cannot be separated from these micro-cultural processes themselves. Since the purpose of this project was the design of activities, there was a need for flexibility between activities. Therefore it became important for the researcher to be also a teacher and participant in the play. The project takes inspiration from video-ethnography as “a way of researching from the perspective of being a part of an environment rather than from that of asking someone to tell you about it in spoken words” (Pink, 2014, p. 106). Video-analysis also includes data in the form of body-language, something of great importance when working with children ages three to five years old, where body language is an important way of communicating and provides data that the children cannot yet verbalise. Also, the dynamics and swiftness of change of focus in a group of three-year-old children requires videorecordings for more detailed analysis. Participatory data collection allows for exploring the process of learning as a collective effort (Pink, 2014). Ethnography especially considers all different kinds of knowledge, and all different kinds of data are included such as drawings, photos and artwork. Analysis (the organisation) of data is a continuous process that is initiated immediately as data collection begins. The teacher/researcher in this case is an educated preschool teacher, who also holds a Master degree in developmental psychology.
The theoretical chemistry, i.e. the chemistry concepts behind the practical methods, is not described here, but can be found in Table 4. In addition, only the headlines of the emergent science skills are included here; for further content, see Table 4. Suggestions for educational tools in activities are also included. The core concept is displayed in the middle of the image (Fig. 2) as it is in focus for all practical methods and connects directly to most of the emergent skills.
![]() | ||
Fig. 1 An example of an individual activity design including emergent science skills and the remaining considerations from scaffolding. |
Within each of the activities, the core content that all things are made of small particles was in focus.
The main content of the activities for the sessions presented here involved mainly observation skills. As the children were three years old and had a limited language repertoire, a simplification of the content of discussions and body language was made, such that all of the content, including body language gestures, were seen as either belonging to everyday word/concept or science word/concept. An example of an everyday concept expressed by body language was the connection between movement and snowflakes that came up during the first session. All children recognised a picture of a snowflake and when they were asked to show snowflakes, some children fell to the ground and some started running across the room being snowflakes in a snowstorm.
Categories for the quality markers for play-based learning activities (Fleer and Pramling, 2015) were made together with focus and interruptions as a separate category. In all, 10 different categories were developed (see Table 5).
Category | Definitions | Example |
---|---|---|
Creating common ground within interruptions (teacher) | The teacher purposely tries to redirect children's attention towards the activity | “Do you remember why we are doing this?” |
Narrator (teacher) | Moments when the teacher leads the session | Telling a story, asking questions… |
Science (teacher) | Moments when the teacher specifically introduces scientific words or concepts | “This is a magnifying glass and we use it for looking at very small things.” |
Affective imagination (children) | Combines imagination emotionally connected with feelings | “It looks maybe… like strawberries. I love strawberries!” |
In/out of Imagination (children) | “Flickering” between reality and fantasy | “That is not a real man, it is just a statue!” |
Collective mind (children + teacher) | Collective discussions, common ground | “It is a tea bag. Do your parents drink tea? Yes!” |
Everyday word (children) | Word meaning not established or word meaning established | Cat or chair either pointing at a cat or a chair or just using the word |
Everyday concept (children) | Involves (or contains) everyday content. Shows meaning and additional experiences. | “Jump! Falling! |
Once I fell down and hurt myself, I got a bandage. If you climb high, you can hurt yourself.” |
Focus describes the times when all children were focused on the activity at hand, and interruptions marked a complete non-activity related change of focus such as “my grandfather is coming to see me” or leaving the activity to dance for a while. The choice of using interruptions as a category was made to allow for evaluating what types of activities caught these children's attention.
Teacher narrator was another category that is seen as a quality marker for play-based learning. This category includes the moments when the teacher is leading the activity through storytelling or asking questions. Science (teacher) is a transformation of what Fleer refers to as “science models” and includes those moments when the teacher “makes science visible” (Fleer, 2009b). The transformation was made only for the purpose of separating science introduced by the teacher from science introduced by children.
Affective imagination describes those moments that include emotions together with scientific concepts, scientific words, everyday concepts, or everyday words. Imagination is here seen as depending on everyday concepts or words (Vygotsky, 2004).
Collective mind, or common ground, describes the moments where all parties were focused on and discussed the same thing, as a meeting of minds. Creating common ground is a category added to analyse how many times and how the teacher/researcher drew the children's attention back after interruptions. Sustained shared science thinking or co-construction of scientific knowledge is here seen as a combination of scientific concepts and collective mind.
Content (children) was defined as children's everyday and scientific concepts or words. Examples are provided in Table 5. Here, only the conversations of children are included; blanks show pauses in conversation. As science skills are integrated into what is here referred to as science, observation skills such as defining flavours as sweet or salty are also classified here as scientific words.
The categorisations more focused on content were then further evaluated in the analysis as described in Table 6. It was here essential to determine if sustained shared thinking or sustained science thinking was indeed achieved.
Category | Content of category | Code | Combination of category and content |
---|---|---|---|
Interruptions | Focus on non-related issue | 0 | |
Focus on activity | 1 | ||
Creating common ground | 2 | ||
Affective imagination (children) | No affective imagination | 0 | |
Affective imagination | 1 | ||
In/out of imagination (children) | Out of imagination | 0 | |
In imagination | 1 | ||
Content (children) | — | 0 | |
Everyday word | 1 | ||
Everyday concept | 2 | ||
Science word | 3 | ||
Science concept | 4 | ||
Collective mind (teacher + children) | — | 0 | |
Everyday word | 1 | Sustained shared thinking | |
Everyday concept | 2 | Sustained shared science thinking | |
Science word | 3 | ||
Science concept | 4 | ||
Narrator (teacher) | No everyday concept | 0 | |
Everyday concept | 1 | ||
Science (teacher) | No science concept | 0 | |
Science concept | 1 |
After categorisation and marking duration times, the categories were scored with either zero or one. One meant successful. For example, the number one for the category of focus meant that focus was obtained. The number zero meant that focus was lost. The exception to this valuation was content (children) and collective mind where all received a number each; see (Table 6). For this particular evaluation, no additional quality judgements (except for 1 and 0) were made. Duration times were then transferred to Excel, where each segment was compared to the duration of the entire session and percentages of the segments were calculated. These percentages were then transferred to Python.org (2019), a programming language that transformed the calculated percentages into graphs.
Session description | Location | Duration (minutes) |
---|---|---|
The story of the king | Sofa | 9 |
Magnifying glass | Arts and crafts area | 9 |
Macro-pictures and collected items | Arts and crafts area | 9 |
Drawing, finding the sugar | Dining area | 35 |
Tasting the white powders | Dining area | 25 |
Filtering | Activity area | 22 |
Making chocolate balls, filtering | Activity area | 20 |
The sets of data were analysed for Focus, here visualised with the colour green and Interruptions, here marked in purple. These two points of analysis were made to find out if the design did indeed capture the children's interest (Fig. 3). Added to this analysis is the category of Creating common ground, marked with orange.
A high percentage of focus was observed for the different activities. In Session 5, role-play including facial paint was introduced. A higher number of interruptions were observed during this activity and were mainly due to the children arguing over the different kinds of paint or partaking in play jousting. As Fig. 3 shows, sometimes the children themselves returned their focus to the activity at hand and other times, here marked in orange, the teacher/researcher recaptured their interest mainly through asking questions. Results for the category imagination are not displayed in the following summaries, though when comparing the category of “affective imagination” to “moving in and out of imagination”, the discrepancy between the two shows imagination without clearly expressed emotions.
Summaries of results from Sessions 3, 4 and 5 are shown below in Fig. 4–6. These three represent the sessions with the highest, lowest and the average focus time. For all three sessions presented below, only the categories of collective mind and children's content include all four codes, as displayed in Table 6. The colour codes for these two categories are displayed below each of the sessions.
![]() | ||
Fig. 4 Categorisations of the quality markers for conceptual play and their relative duration times as a percentage of the entire session are displayed. Example provided from Session 3. |
![]() | ||
Fig. 5 Categorisations of the quality markers for conceptual play and their relative duration times as a percentage of the entire session are displayed. Example provided from Session 4. |
![]() | ||
Fig. 6 Categorisations of the quality markers for conceptual play and their relative duration times as a percentage of the entire session are displayed. Example provided from Session 5. |
Affective imagination occurred less frequently and was found difficult to maintain over all of the activities (which can be seen in all three of the activities presented here). The children began working with the actual task and they moved out of play as they were no longer princesses or princes, although actively participating in the task at hand. When analysing affective imagination and in and out of imagination for this session, both categories were less frequent. After the initial analysis, the reason for this was found to be partly contextual, since the children had been taught to be careful, and not to play, in the arts and craft area. Maria addresses the issue, “We don’t play here” (referring to the arts and crafts area).
Teacher/researcher – Ok, maybe, but we need the sugar for whom?
Maria – For the cake.
Teacher/researcher – For whose cake?
John – The king's cake!
This activity also included dissolving, as one way to differentiate the white powders from each other. The teacher asked guided questions to make the children hypothesise about what happens to the sugar when dissolved into water, and one child answered:
Teacher/researcher – Is the powder gone?
John – Ah.
Teacher/researcher – But, where did it go? Have you seen it?
John – They went in between each other (sugar and water, author's note).
When analysing data for creating common ground or collective mind, we found that the story of the king was indeed helpful for a meeting of minds, but equally useful were recollections of activities or questions concerning science content or everyday concepts. The topic of birthdays was easy to relate to, and everyday concepts were frequently reoccurring as the children picked up on each other's comments and extended them with their own experiences.
As previously mentioned, the state of the children's language development limited their contributions to the category of children's content. Science (teacher) concepts were few in these activities, and this was partially due to the decision to create further experiences and develop word understanding for observation skills. Analysing children's science concepts or words and how they appear between activities lies beyond the scope of this part of the study. Nonetheless, observation skills were easily adopted and implemented to such an extent that it became difficult to separate them from everyday concepts and they were frequently reoccurring in-between activities. This shows how easily motive can be founded at this level of education. Sustained shared thinking was easily established, although sustained shared science thinking was more difficult to achieve, mainly due to the focus on observation skills. The foundations for higher-order thinking or theoretical knowledge are described by Vygotsky (2004) as dependent on being able to move between the abstract and the concrete. The analysis of the category of in and out of imagination did show a natural flow of connections between everyday and science concepts, suggesting that these children were indeed well in the process of creating theoretical knowledge.
Vi leker inte här
Session 4 excerpts in original language (Swedish):
Som godis
osynlig
Session 5 excerpts in original language (Swedish):
Lärare/Forskaren – Ok, kanske, men vi behöver sockret, till vem?
Maria – För tårtan
Lärare/forskare – Vems tårta?
John – Kungens tårta!
Läraren/Forskaren – Är pulvret borta?
John – Ah.
Lärarn/Forskaren – Men vart tog det vägen? Har du sett det?
John – De gick mellan varann (sockret och vattnet)
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2019 |