James
Ross
*,
Leslie
Nuñez
and
Chinh Chu
Lai
Department of Chemistry, East Los Angeles College, Monterey Park, California 91754, USA. E-mail: rossj2@elac.edu
First published on 10th July 2018
Students’ decisions to enter or persist in STEM courses is linked with their affective domain. The influence of factors impacting students’ affective domain in introductory college chemistry classes, such as attitude, is often overlooked by instructors, who instead focus on students’ mathematical abilities as sole predictors of academic achievement. The current academic barrier to enrollment in introductory college chemistry classes is typically a passing grade in a mathematics prerequisite class. However, mathematical ability is only a piece of the puzzle in predicting preparedness for college chemistry. Herein, students’ attitude toward the subject of chemistry was measured using the original Attitudes toward the Subject of Chemistry Inventory (ASCI). Partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) was used to chart and monitor the development of students’ attitude toward the subject of chemistry during an introductory college chemistry course. Results from PLS-SEM support a 3-factor (intellectual accessibility, emotional satisfaction, and interest and utility) structure, which could signal the distinct cognitive, affective, and behavioral components of attitude, according to its theoretical tripartite framework. Evidence of a low-involvement hierarchy of attitude effect is also presented herein. This study provides a pathway for instructors to identify at-risk students, exhibiting low affective characteristics, early in a course so that academic interventions are feasible. The results presented here have implications for the design and implementation of teaching strategies geared toward optimizing student achievement in introductory college chemistry.
Assessment of students’ logical thinking ability has been shown to mimic the correlation between mathematical ability and chemistry achievement. Bunce and Hutchinson (1993) identified the Group Assessment of Logical Thinking (GALT) test as a predictor of academic success in college chemistry. They found that scores on the GALT test can be used as an alternative to mathematical SAT scores to predict success in chemistry because both the SAT and GALT measure students’ ability to do mathematical manipulations. For students who are non-science majors, GALT scores may even serve as a better predictor of success than mathematical SAT scores because non-science major chemistry courses often include less mathematical manipulation than a chemistry course designed for science majors. Lewis and Lewis (2007) employed the Test of Logical Thinking (TOLT) over a three year period to identify at-risk individuals among a population of over 3000 students entering general chemistry. The TOLT was able to correctly predict at-risk students with 70.5% accuracy compared with 72.5% accuracy for the SAT and was shown to satisfactorily predict at-risk students for whom SAT scores are unavailable.
It is possible not to observe each distinct component of attitude during attitude research, and hence not to observe any hierarchy of attitude effect. For example, the three components of attitude can converge into one component when the inter-component correlations are very high. This can occur where reporting of attitudes is exclusively verbal, and where the attitudinal object is not physically present during the reporting of attitude (Breckler, 1984). It is also possible that the inter-components of attitude are completely independent of each other (Zajonc, 1980). Since a hierarchy of attitude effect specifies the causal direction between the three sub-components of attitude, all three sub-components must be independent of each other and must be measureable in the data to enable an analysis of a hierarchy of attitude effect. Extant attitude theories like the theory of reasoned action (TRA) and the theory of planned behavior (TPB) consider affect, behavior and cognition to be somewhat positively correlated, whether or not correlations are observed in the data (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975; Ajzen, 1991).
The CAB hierarchy is considered high-involvement due to its order of influence and its behavioral culmination, which testifies that the individual places a high personal value on the attitudinal object in question. An individual following the CAB hierarchy regarding an attitudinal object invests time and effort researching and accumulating personal knowledge about the attitudinal object (cognition). This research is used to formulate feelings toward the attitudinal object (affect), which are then translated into either covert behavioral decision (behavioral intention) or overt behavioral action (behavior) (Lavidge and Steiner, 1961; Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975; Ray, 1982). Since behavior can be considered the least reversible and hence most committal component of the tripartite structure of attitude, the placement of behavior (or behavioral intention) as the culminating component of attitude solidifies the CAB hierarchy as high-involvement to the operating individual. Students exhibiting the CAB hierarchy would be the ideal outcome since it would indicate their association of a high value with chemistry toward their overall education. It would follow that these students will probably be committed to meaningful learning and will be engaged in their chemistry class.
The ABC hierarchy could be considered to cater to an individual's need for self-gratification with regards to an attitudinal object. The antecedence of affect in the ABC hierarchy testifies to the importance of an emotional reward for the individual regarding the attitudinal object (Solomon, 1997). It is unlikely that the ABC hierarchy would be displayed by students in an education setting where chemistry is the attitudinal object. On the contrary, the ABC hierarchy would be more likely to surface where the attitudinal object was a personal gift for the individual, such as a new computer or a new car.
The CBA hierarchy is low-involvement due to the removal of behavior (or behavioral intention) as the culminating act of the attitude structure and its replacement with the affective component. The CBA hierarchy signifies that the individual does not place a high personal value on the attitudinal object in question (Krugman, 1965; Ray, 1982). An individual following the CBA hierarchy when considering an attitudinal object is initially investing time conducting the appropriate research and is acquiring relevant knowledge regarding the attitudinal object. However, the individual then prematurely makes a covert behavioral decision or overtly acts in an arguably ill-advised manner. It is only upon later reflection of the premature behavioral impulse that the individual begins to develop feelings about the behavior toward the attitudinal object. Students’ adoption of the CBA hierarchy would be considered the least desirable outcome and should spark an ethical need for a remedial academic attitudinal intervention. It could follow that these students will probably be unengaged with meaningful learning or perhaps simply do not see the need or benefit to learn chemistry.
Evaluation of students’ affective domain to garner insights into their learning in college chemistry classes is an underused but potentially profitable educational strategy (Chan and Bauer, 2014; Kahveci and Orgill, 2015). Numerous studies have probed the different aspects of students’ affective domain, such as attitudes (Cukrowska et al., 1999; Osborne et al., 2003; Salta and Tzougraki, 2004; Grove and Bretz, 2007; Barbera et al., 2008; Cheung, 2009; Heredia and Lewis, 2012; Else-Quest et al., 2013), self-concept (Wilkins, 2004; Bauer, 2005; Lewis et al., 2009; Nielsen and Yezierski, 2015), and self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997; Kan and Akbas, 2006; Uzuntiryaki and Aydin, 2009; Ferrell and Barbera, 2015; Vishnumolakala et al., 2017). In general, a positive affective domain leads to higher academic achievement in science than a negative affective domain.
A potential distraction from the dissemination of research findings concerning students’ attitude toward chemistry is the internal inconsistencies within the literature, arguably owing to overreaching conclusions made from research using blunt instruments that fail to adequately disaggregate the composite attitude metric (Adams and Wieman, 2011). For example, student attitude is sometimes erroneously considered as a single construct, and its tripartite composite structure (ABC) is ignored. There is a call for standardized rigor in educational research that use survey instruments, especially the uniform confirmation of the reliability and validity of data collected by instruments prior to the drawing of conclusions (Arjoon et al., 2013).
Students at two year and four year colleges possibly maintain different attitudes toward chemistry, and there is no logical reason to think that their attitudinal measurements should be the same. Institutional funding, location, entrance policies and reputation are all potential influencers of attitude (Malcom and Feder, 2016). Indeed, the results from an educational research instrument performed in one four year institution do not necessarily predict the outcomes when the instrument is used in another four year institution. For example, Xu and Lewis (2011) found that the ASCI instrument in their hands and with their students performed differently when compared with Bauer's original work (2008).
A present gap in the literature is the lack of research looking at students’ attitude toward chemistry in two year institutions. Perhaps this is due to concerns about the small class sizes often prevailing in two year institutions and the problems of performing structural equation modeling on small sample sizes; other reasons for the lack of attention at the two year level could of course prevail. Another current gap in the literature concerning attitudes toward chemistry is the lack of research considering the entire complement of the tripartite attitude model. Studies have looked at cognition and affect without behavior (or behavioral intention) and have justified the absence of behavior (or behavioral intention) on grounds of a conflict with course grade that was also measured (Xu and Lewis, 2011). By not including the full complement of cognition, affect and behavior (covert or overt) in research of students’ attitude toward chemistry, researchers have thus far been unable to investigate the hierarchy of attitude effects. Furthermore, the absence of the behavioral component from these attitude studies could introduce a psychometric bias to the results obtained.
CFA provides a variety of fit indices that are used to critique a proposed structural equation model. For example, large chi-square (χ2) values, comparative fit index (CFI) scores greater than 0.95, and standardized root-mean-squared (SRMR) scores less than 0.08 are all considered to be markers of good-fitting structural models of data (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Guidelines for the implementation of CB-SEM in educational research support the relevance of reporting these fit indices (Schreiber et al., 2006; Schreiber, 2008; Kline, 2015).
CFA has become one of the most commonly used statistical procedures in applied research because it is well equipped to address the types of questions that researchers often ask (Brown, 2014). CFA permits the testing of competing structural models of data, helps researchers navigate toward increasingly parsimonious interpretations of results, and has been used to produce nomological networks of variables affecting the learning of science, albeit cautiously (Blalock, 1986).
In PLS-SEM, latent constructs are considered as either exogenous or endogenous during the evaluation of structural models, as is the case in CB-SEM. Exogenous constructs are latent variables that do not have any structural path input from other latent constructs, whereas endogenous constructs are target latent variables that are explained by other latent constructs in the structural model (Hair et al., 2016). In the formative measurement mode, where arrows point from the factors to the items, error terms exist. However, in the reflective measurement mode, where arrows point from the items to the latent constructs, error terms are not present due to the inherent composite formulation of the constructs in PLS-SEM. PLS-SEM softens the assumption in CB-SEM of a common factor causation to explain all covariation between sets of indicators. Instead, PLS-SEM represents constructs with proxies, which are weighted composites of indicator variables (Rigdon, 2012).
Contrary to CB-SEM, PLS-SEM has no accepted global measure of goodness-of-fit index (Cudeck, 1989; Hu and Bentler, 1999; Hair et al., 2011; Henseler and Sarstedt, 2013). Therefore, the concept of “fit index” does not translate in the same way to PLS-SEM as it does to CB-SEM (Chin, 2010). Key criteria for assessing structural models with PLS-SEM include the significance of path coefficients, β, the size of R2 values for endogenous constructs, the f2 and q2 effect sizes, and the predictive relevance values, Q2 (Stone, 1974; Geisser, 1975). The predictive relevance of endogenous latent constructs in PLS-SEM, as registered by their Q2 values, is obtained following the blindfolding procedure. Blindfolding systematically deletes data points at a given omission distance (D) between 5 and 12 and then predicts their value using the remaining data. For example, setting D equal to 5 would cycle the blindfolding procedure five times until every data point has been omitted and predicted. Differences between omitted data and predicted data are used to calculate Q2 values (Hair et al., 2016). This is why PLS-SEM is said to be predictive in nature.
Whereas neither PLS-SEM nor CB-SEM is generally considered to be superior to the other, under certain circumstances PLS-SEM can often furnish more usable and hence useful results compared with the results from CB-SEM from comparable input data (Hair et al., 2011). For example, PLS-SEM is insensitive to the skewness and kurtosis of its input data and can return robust and dependable model results from data lacking a normal distribution (non-parametric). Regardless, researchers often report skewness and kurtosis results even when using PLS-SEM. In contrast, CB-SEM requires normally distributed data to produce dependable model results. PLS-SEM can accommodate single-item latent constructs whereas this is not possible in CB-SEM. PLS-SEM has greater statistical power compared to CB-SEM, meaning that PLS-SEM is more likely to correctly identify significant structural relationships than CB-SEM. Furthermore, PLS-SEM can handle greater model complexity compared to CB-SEM (Hair et al., 2016). PLS-SEM is the method of choice for exploratory research analyses that aim to extend existing structural theoretical models and provide more robust statistical results with smaller sample sizes, rendering PLS-SEM suited to structural equation modeling of students’ attitude toward chemistry in two year institutions (Wold, 1982; Reinartz et al., 2009; Hair et al., 2014).
Expecting factor loadings from an attitude measuring instrument such as ASCI, developed in a four year institution housing traditional students, to predict the attitudinal outcomes of minority and non-traditional students surveyed using the same instrument in two year colleges is not obviously reasonable. Presuming that positive attitudes toward chemistry correlate with achievement in chemistry, and with an ultimate view to augment student achievement in introductory chemistry in our institution, the following research questions were considered:
(1) Does the original ASCI instrument, developed and administered in traditional four year institutions serving predominantly non-minority students, reveal any meaningful attitude constructs when administered to the predominantly minority students at an urban two year college?
(2) Can PLS-SEM offer a meaningful interpretation of the students’ attitude data, in line with the tripartite attitude model?
(3) Can path analysis using PLS-SEM reveal a hierarchy of attitude effect in our students’ data?
The number of useable completed surveys from weeks 1 and 5 was 98 and 85, respectively. To be considered useable for week 1, surveys had to be completed by the same student twice during week 1. 20 surveys were disregarded from week 1 due to students failing to repeat the survey on two separate occasions. To be considered useable for week 5, surveys had to be completed by students who had previously taken the survey during week 1. A further loss of 13 more surveys by week 5 resulted from student attrition. Student participants in week 1 surveys were 50.0% female, 50.0% male, and 76.5%, 20.4% and 3.1% were classified as Hispanic/Latino, Asian/Pacific Islander, and other, respectively. Students taking week 5 surveys were 52.9% female, 47.1% male, and 63.5%, 29.4% and 7.1% were classified as Hispanic/Latino, Asian/Pacific Islander, and other, respectively.
All students’ survey data was manually input into Excel spreadsheets (Bauer, 2008). EFA of the internal structure of the ASCI instrument in our hands was performed with XLSTAT 2017.5. The original literature reports used principal components analysis with varimax rotation (Bauer, 2008; Xu and Lewis, 2011). More recent literature reports used principal axis factoring and direct oblimin rotation (Brown, 2014; Kahveci, 2015). Both rotation methods were evaluated, and since trivial difference was observed between varimax and oblimin rotations, the former was used in this study.
In deciding how many factors to extract from ASCI, the eigenvalue-greater-than-one rule was adopted, along with inspection of the scree plot, and careful consideration of the interpretability of results against the ABC model of attitude (Rosenberg and Hovland, 1960). Predictive structural modeling (PLS-SEM) of students’ data was performed using XLSTAT 2017.5. The statistical significance of structural paths was established using 5000 bootstrap samples. The predictive relevance of each construct was obtained from the blindfolding procedure (Hair et al., 2016).
Analysis of item scores showed good distribution normality. Skewness and kurtosis values were less than ±2 except for item 2 surveyed during week 1 (Appendix 1). As shown in Appendix 2, Cronbach's alpha values for the ASCI constructs (excluding fear, as this was only a single item construct) from the original study were at or above the threshold satisfactory value of 0.70, ranging from 0.70 to 0.78, and resemble literature values (Davidshofer and Murphy, 2005; Sijtsma, 2009). Cronbach's alpha values ranging from 0.78 to 0.83 were originally reported by Bauer (2008), while values ranging from 0.71 to 0.82 were reported by Xu and Lewis (2011). Test-retest reliability correlations for the ASCI survey ranged from 0.55 to 0.72 and resemble the correlation values originally reported by Bauer (2008), which ranged from 0.64 to 0.74 (Appendix 2). This signals that students’ attitude responses during the first week of class were adequately stable.
The suitability of survey results for factor analysis is often determined prior to performing EFA. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test of sampling adequacy measures the proportion of variance among survey items that might be due to a common variance. KMO = (∑rij2)/[(∑rij2) + (∑μij2)] for i ≠ j, where rij2 are the correlation coefficients and μij2 are the partial correlation coefficients. Lower proportions of variance (where ∑μij2 is minimized) are more suitable for FA and are converted to a value between 0 and 1, with values >0.70 indicating an acceptable KMO value for FA and structure detection (Kaiser, 1974). The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy for the 20-item ASCI administered during week 1 and 5 was 0.801 and 0.856, respectively, signaling the suitability of our data for FA.
Bartlett's test of sphericity addresses the hypothesis that the correlation matrix is an identity matrix. If confirmed true (p > 0.05), the data is unrelated and further analysis for detecting structure is pointless. Bartlett's test of sphericity for our data was significant (week 1: χ2(209) = 1069.297, p < 0.0001; week 5: χ2(209) = 1143.849, p < 0.0001) and signifies that our students’ attitude data is correlated, allowing rejection of the null hypothesis. Together, the KMO and Bartlett's test results imply that the ASCI data from our students is reliable and factor analysis of the student data is feasible.
In our hands, the intellectual accessibility construct was for the most part loaded onto one factor and seems to be captured in its entirety without overlapping other constructs by a variety of students in different institutions (Bauer, 2008; Xu and Lewis, 2011). In general, however, the ASCI instrument performed differently with our students compared with Bauer's students, but performed similarly to Xu and Lewis’ students and Brown's students (Xu and Lewis, 2011; Brown et al., 2014). For example, our data revealed factors containing a mixture of items from Bauer's interest and utility, emotional satisfaction and anxiety constructs as reported in Brown's and Xu and Lewis’ work, whereas this was not observed by Bauer; instead, anxiety was a cleanly resolved factor (Bauer, 2008). We speculate that the cultural and ethnicity differences between our students and the students used to validate the instrument originally are contributing to the handling subtleties of the ASCI instrument (Xu et al., 2015). The alternative factor, values, which was a construct molded by Brown et al. (2014) from a combination of items belonging to intellectual accessibility, interest and utility, anxiety, and emotional satisfaction constructs was also not observed as an independent construct from our students’ data.
The unexpected overlap and common factor loading of items belonging to the interest and utility and emotional satisfaction constructs was previously observed in Bauer's original work, by Xu and Lewis, and by Brown et al., and could indicate that all our students’ interest and utility measurements are being forged by emotional sentiments (Bauer, 2008; Xu and Lewis, 2011; Brown et al., 2014). To our knowledge, these authors did not pursue the possibility that overlapping factor loadings and significant correlations between factors could enable the interpretation of a hierarchy of attitude effect (Beatty and Kahle, 1988; Riketta, 2008).
In the presence of the interest and utility items, coupling with emotional satisfaction items occurs and could indicate the presence of a composite latent structure. As far as we can tell, Bauer did not consider this specific possibility in his original work (2008) and instead just concluded that emotional satisfaction was not a totally independent measure. Xu and Lewis (2011) in their work to modify the ASCI and produce a shorter and potentially psychometrically superior survey, ASCIv2, dismissed the interest and utility factor claiming it is undesirable for one subscale to have more than one concept. However, an alternative possibility, not considered by Xu and Lewis, is that Bauer's original label (interest and utility) could be replaced by a label invoking just one concept, such as behavioral intention. Since the original construct interest and utility has already been demonstrated by others to produce reliable and valid data, all that would be needed to justify its relabeling as behavioral intention is evidence that its items could be interpreted as a behavioral intention construct. Some authors contend that the behavioral component of attitude is not easily captured with accuracy with a self-reporting instrument, like ASCI (Xu and Lewis, 2011). Perhaps this is why Bauer chose not to attempt to measure behavior or behavioral intention using the ASCI instrument.
According to Ajzen's theory of planned behavior (TPB), behavioral intentions capture the motivational influencers of a behavior (Ajzen, 1985). The decision to represent behavioral intention by the items in Bauer's interest and utility construct follows from the realization that Bauer used motivational descriptors as the semantic opposites in this construct. For example, identifying an attitudinal object as “worthwhile” or “beneficial” would reasonably be expected to result in a motivational incentive to consider a behavioral action (Ajzen, 1991). Additional support for the decision to represent behavioral intention by the items in the interest and utility construct comes from Eccles et al.'s expectancy-value of achievement motivation model, which links an individual's behavioral intention to subjective task value. The four motivational components of subjective task value outlined in Eccles et al.'s model are importance, cost, and notably interest and usefulness (Eccles et al., 2007).
Following EFA of the ASCI survey results, intellectual accessibility items 1, 4 and 5 were assigned to cognition, emotional satisfaction items 7, 11 and 14 were considered representative of affect, and interest and utility items 2, 3, 6, 12 and 15 were assigned to behavioral intention. Intellectual accessibility items 9 and 10 were dropped from further analysis due to cautionary loading concerns found by others (Xu and Lewis, 2011; Xu et al., 2015). Emotional satisfaction item 17 was dropped from further analysis due to loading concerns from our EFA. Removal of one or two items representing a latent construct is not expected to significantly affect the analysis of the structural equation model since the construct is still represented by the remaining items (Hair et al., 2016).
Average scores for the constituent items considered for the tripartite structural model ranged from 3.388 to 5.816, and from 3.129 to 5.682, for week 1 and 5, respectively (1–7 range, 4 midpoint). Higher scores indicate more positive attitudes (Table 1). The highest and lowest score was found for ASCI item 2 and 4 in both week 1 and week 5 surveys, indicating that students find chemistry beneficial and complicated during the course. ASCI items 1, 4, and 5 scored below the midpoint value of 4, suggesting that students find chemistry intellectually inaccessible. ASCI items 7, 11, and 14 scored above 4 in value, suggesting that students find chemistry emotionally satisfying. ASCI items 2, 3, 6, 12, and 15 scored above 4 in value (except item 3 in week 1) and were the highest scores, suggesting that students find chemistry interesting and useful above all else. It is an encouraging sign that our students value the usefulness of the subject of chemistry and bodes well for efforts to increase the number of STEM majors at our institution.
Construct | Item number | Item | Mean (SD)a | Mean (SD)b | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
a Week 1 (N = 98). b Week 5 (N = 85). c Item reversed here to aid interpretation; negatively stated item is reversed before averaging. | |||||
Cognition | 1c | Hard | Easy | 3.388 (1.352) | 3.624 (1.543) |
4 | Complicated | Simple | 3.388 (1.469) | 3.129 (1.454) | |
5 | Confusing | Clear | 3.878 (1.594) | 3.612 (1.423) | |
Behavioral intention | 2 | Worthless | Beneficial | 5.816 (1.409) | 5.682 (1.071) |
3c | Boring | Exciting | 4.571 (1.741) | 5.000 (1.447) | |
6c | Bad | Good | 5.337 (1.631) | 5.224 (1.199) | |
12c | Dull | Interesting | 5.602 (1.504) | 5.612 (1.273) | |
15c | Useless | Worthwhile | 5.429 (1.392) | 5.118 (1.418) | |
Affect | 7c | Frustrating | Satisfying | 4.214 (1.508) | 4.529 (1.615) |
11c | Unpleasant | Pleasant | 4.490 (1.203) | 4.729 (1.409) | |
14c | Uncomfortable | Comfortable | 4.235 (1.208) | 4.376 (1.431) |
Brandriet et al. (2013) have shown that superior structural models of students’ attitude are found later in a course, rather than at the beginning, when attitudes have had chance to establish and settle. Although this finding is to be expected and can be of use, it could be argued that surveying students’ attitude toward chemistry near the end of a course offers no time for meaningful intervention, if that is the reason for measuring students’ attitude in the first place (Abdullah et al., 2009; Shaw, 2012; Koponen et al., 2012; Vishnumolakala et al., 2017). However, in pursuit of the best model, we compared structural models from data taken at the beginning and end of the course.
Latent variable | Internal consistency reliability | |
---|---|---|
Composite reliability | Cronbach's alpha | |
a Week 5 (N = 85). | ||
Cognition | 0.890 | 0.815 |
Behavioral intention | 0.915 | 0.879 |
Affect | 0.860 | 0.749 |
Item | Latent variable | ||
---|---|---|---|
Cognition | Behavioral intention | Affect | |
a Week 5 (N = 85). Loadings are in bold. b Negatively stated. | |||
1b | 0.911 | 0.260 | 0.545 |
4 | 0.776 | −0.028 | 0.256 |
5 | 0.816 | 0.164 | 0.403 |
2 | 0.144 | 0.726 | 0.527 |
3b | 0.201 | 0.864 | 0.689 |
6b | 0.088 | 0.821 | 0.601 |
12b | 0.227 | 0.834 | 0.700 |
15b | 0.200 | 0.848 | 0.638 |
7b | 0.452 | 0.691 | 0.896 |
11b | 0.471 | 0.653 | 0.838 |
14b | 0.386 | 0.556 | 0.702 |
Cognition | Behavioral intention | Affect | AVE | |
---|---|---|---|---|
a Week 5 (N = 85). | ||||
Cognition | 1.000 | — | — | 0.699 |
Behavioral intention | 0.049 | 1.000 | — | 0.673 |
Affect | 0.284 | 0.606 | 1.000 | 0.666 |
The Fornell–Larcker criterion (FLC) for discriminant validity was inspected (Table 4). The FLC is used to address and prevent multicollinearity issues. The FLC is a common way to evaluate the degree of shared variance between latent constructs in a structural model. The FLC is adhered to when the average variance extracted (AVE) value of each latent construct exceeds the constructs’ highest squared correlation with other latent constructs (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). All AVE values were higher than the squared correlations with other latent variables and ranged from 0.666 to 0.699. Together, these results demonstrate that discriminant validity was achieved for the latent constructs under consideration.
Model | Construct | Adjusted R2 | f 2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Affect | Behavioral intention | Cognition | |||
a Week 5 (N = 85). Large adjusted R2 values suggests that a model has in-sample predictive relevance for an endogenous construct. f2 is the effect size of adjusted R2. Values for f2 of 0.02, 0.15 and 0.35 are viewed as small, medium and large effects, respectively. | |||||
2 | Affect | 0.284 | — | 1.792 | — |
Behavioral intention | 0.655 | — | — | — | |
Cognition | — | 0.397 | 0.155 | — | |
4 | Affect | — | — | 1.540 | 0.536 |
Behavioral intention | 0.606 | — | — | 0.155 | |
Cognition | 0.373 | — | — | — | |
6 | Affect | 0.741 | — | — | — |
Behavioral intention | 0.041 | 1.792 | — | — | |
Cognition | — | 0.536 | 0.051 | — | |
7 | Affect | 0.738 | — | — | — |
Behavioral intention | — | 1.830 | — | — | |
Cognition | — | 0.527 | — | — | |
8 | Affect | — | — | 1.635 | — |
Behavioral intention | 0.648 | — | — | — | |
Cognition | — | — | 0.126 | — |
![]() | ||
Fig. 2 Alternative PLS-SEM structural models from ASCI data recorded during week 5 ((7) CBA hierarchy; (8) CAB hierarchy; N = 85). *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001. |
The high-involvement CAB and the ABC hierarchy effect models are represented in models 2 or 8 and 4, respectively. The high-involvement hierarchy model 2 shows that approximately 66% of students’ behavioral intention is predicted by a combination of their cognition and affect (Fig. 1). The largest f2 effect size in model 2 is between affect and behavioral intention (1.792), as was the case in model 6 (Table 5). Model 8 was also constructed whereby the path connecting cognition and behavioral intention was eliminated (Fig. 2). This more parsimonious high-involvement hierarchy model maintained most of the adjusted R2 value of the behavioral intention construct (0.648) and maintained its large f2 effect size (1.635) found in model 2 (Table 5). Model 8 could be a better representative model of a high-involvement CAB hierarchy effect compared with model 2. Inspection of the models presented in Fig. 2 shows that cognition and behavioral intention explain the variance in affect (74%, model 7) better than cognition and affect can explain the variance in behavioral intention (65%, model 8).
In the ABC hierarchy model 4 (Fig. 1), only 37% of the terminal attitude construct, cognition, is predicted from the combination of students’ affect and behavioral intention. On theory grounds, it is unlikely that the ABC hierarchy model is an appropriate model for our students’ attitude toward the subject of chemistry data, and its reduced in-sample predictive qualities (adjusted R2) support this assertion (Table 5).
Bootstrapping was used to assess the significance of the path coefficients (β) in each model (Table 6). Although most path coefficients are significant at the p < 0.05 level, the paths between cognition and behavioral intention in models 6 and 8 both exhibit a 95% confidence interval (CI) range which includes zero. Paths with a 95% CI range which includes zero should be rejected even with p < 0.05 (Hair et al., 2016). This finding supports the notion that the 4% in-sample prediction of behavioral intention by cognition in model 6 is probably meaningless and supports model 7 as a better representative (than model 6) of the low-involvement (CBA) hierarchy effect. This finding also raises the concern that the predictive path connecting cognition and behavioral intention in high-involvement hierarchy model 2 is likely a pattern in the data and could be meaningless (Table 6) (Kline, 2015). However, an alternative possibility is that the path connecting cognition and behavioral intention in model 2 is providing a suppressor effect to the mediation role of affect between cognition and behavioral intention, as evidenced by the negative β value (−0.272) (Cheung and Lau, 2008).
Model | Pathb | β | 95% CI | t-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
a Critical t-values for a two-tailed test: <1.96 (p > 0.05), 1.96 (p = 0.05), >2.58 (p < 0.001). Paths with a 95% confidence interval (CI) including zero should be rejected even with p < 0.05. b A, affect; BI, behavioral intention; C, cognition. | |||||
2 | C → A | 0.533 | [0.309; 0.667] | 5.743 | 0.000 |
C → BI | −0.272 | [−0.448; −0.101] | −3.571 | <0.001 | |
A → BI | 0.924 | [0.795; 1.045] | 12.122 | 0.000 | |
4 | A → C | 0.918 | [0.655; 1.167] | 6.630 | 0.000 |
BI → C | −0.495 | [−0.785; −0.205] | −3.571 | <0.001 | |
A → BI | 0.779 | [0.676; 0.861] | 11.305 | <0.001 | |
6 | C → A | 0.380 | [0.250; 0.500] | 6.630 | 0.000 |
C → BI | 0.220 | [−0.076; 0.419] | 2.059 | 0.043 | |
BI → A | 0.695 | [0.591; 0.794] | 12.122 | 0.000 | |
7 | C → A | 0.377 | [0.256; 0.504] | 6.576 | 0.000 |
BI → A | 0.703 | [0.605; 0.785] | 12.251 | 0.000 | |
8 | C → BI | −0.256 | [−0.399; 0.070] | −3.212 | 0.002 |
A → BI | 0.922 | [0.695; 1.034] | 11.579 | 0.000 |
Cohen's path analysis was performed to analyze the causal direction between the sub-components of attitude (Cohen et al., 1993). Cohen's path analysis reasons that estimated path coefficients should be as close as possible to the actual path coefficients. This means that total squared errors (TSE) between estimated and actual path coefficients should be minimized. Changes in path direction alter the estimated path coefficients but not the actual path coefficients. Therefore, path connections and their directions are critical for calculating estimated paths in proposed structural models (Sun and Zhang, 2006). A causal direction is supported if it leads to a decrease in TSE. In order to conduct Cohen's path analysis, one important criterion is that there must be a path from every variable to the dependent variable; models 2 and 6 meet this criterion and were subsequently used to compare the hierarchy of attitude effects.
Estimated and actual path coefficients were read directly from the PLS-SEM data from models 2 and 6 in XLSTAT (Table 7). TSE values were 0.0441 and 0.0225 for models 2 and 6, respectively. Therefore, by reversing the path from affect → behavioral intention (model 2) to behavioral intention → affect (model 6), the TSE is changed by (0.0225–0.0441)/0.0441 = −49.0%. The negative sign indicates that the TSE is reduced and hence model 6 is an improvement over model 2. Whether or not the apparent improvement of model 6 over model 2 is statistically significant should be gauged by calculating Cohen's d effect size ((TSEmodel6 − TSEmodel
1)/s, where s is the pooled standard deviation of the TSE values). It is not possible to calculate Cohen's d effect size for our data since our structural models only contain 3 latent constructs and hence we cannot calculate the required pooled standard deviation of the TSE values, s (Cohen, 1988). A fourth latent construct would be necessary to allow calculation of the pooled standard deviation. Nevertheless, the apparent 49.0% reduction in the TSE achieved by transitioning from model 2 to model 6 supports the low-achievement (CBA) hierarchy of attitude effect.
Model | Direct path | Indirect path | Estimated | Actual | TSE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
a Week 5 (N = 85). | |||||
2 | C → B | C → A → B | 0.220 | 0.430 | 0.0441 |
6 | C → A | C → B → A | 0.533 | 0.683 | 0.0225 |
The goal of predictive modeling using PLS-SEM is to produce models with high levels of predictive power (large R2, f2, Q2 and q2 values) that are grounded in theory (Sarstedt et al., 2014). Blindfolding was performed to obtain a cross-validated redundancy (Q2) for the endogenous constructs (Hair et al., 2016). Q2 values greater than 0 suggest that a model has predictive relevance for an endogenous construct, whereas Q2 values below 0 indicate a lack of predictive relevance. Lower R2, f2, Q2 and q2 values for latent constructs in the ABC hierarchy model 4 support the assertion that this hierarchy effect is an unlikely theoretical fit to our students’ attitude toward the subject of chemistry (vide supra). However, it is theoretically reasonable that our students’ data is exhibiting either a high-involvement CAB hierarchy or a low-involvement CBA hierarchy.
Effect size is defined by Kelley and Preacher (2012) as “…a quantitative reflection of the magnitude of some phenomenon that is used for the purpose of addressing a question of interest.” The effect size, q2 = (Qincluded2 − Qexcluded2)/(1 − Qincluded2), was computed. Values for q2 of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35 are viewed as small, medium, and large effects, respectively (Hair et al., 2016). Out of the two parsimonious representative models (models 7 and 8), the highest predictive q2 effect size is observed in low-involvement hierarchy model 7 for behavioral intention on affect (0.621, large). A medium predictive q2 effect size (0.179) is observed in model 7 for cognition on affect (Table 8).
Model | Construct | Q 2 | q 2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Affect | Behavioral intention | Cognition | |||
a Week 5 (N = 85). Q2 > 0 suggests that a model has out-of-sample predictive relevance for an endogenous construct; Q2 < 0 indicates a lack of out-of-sample predictive relevance for an endogenous construct. q2 is the effect size of Q2. Values for q2 of 0.02, 0.15 and 0.35 are viewed as small, medium and large effects, respectively. | |||||
2 | Affect | 0.189 | — | 0.707 | — |
Behavioral intention | 0.444 | — | — | — | |
Cognition | — | — | 0.067 | — | |
4 | Affect | — | — | — | 0.296 |
Behavioral intention | 0.408 | — | — | 0.102 | |
Cognition | 0.266 | — | — | — | |
6 | Affect | 0.495 | — | — | — |
Behavioral intention | 0.033 | 0.628 | — | — | |
Cognition | — | 0.184 | — | — | |
7 | Affect | 0.494 | — | — | — |
Behavioral intention | — | 0.621 | — | — | |
Cognition | — | 0.179 | — | — | |
8 | Affect | — | — | 0.407 | — |
Behavioral intention | 0.437 | — | — | — | |
Cognition | — | — | 0.053 | — |
Thus, our students’ data should exhibit a greater predictive link between behavioral intention and affect than between cognition and affect, showing that behavioral intention is driving affect to a greater extent than cognition is driving affect. In high-involvement hierarchy model 8, a large yet lower predictive q2 effect size (0.407) is found for affect on behavioral intention, and a small predictive q2 effect size (0.053) is found for cognition on behavioral intention. The q2 effect size of affect on behavioral intention is greatly increased from 0.407 to 0.707 by reconnecting the path between cognition and behavioral intention and returning to model 2 (Table 8). Overall, it seems that the high-involvement CAB hierarchy is best argued using model 2, while the low-involvement hierarchy is best argued using model 7. Having considered all of the evidence described above, we believe that the findings support the low-involvement hierarchy model 7 as better able to predict our students’ attitude data compared with high-involvement hierarchy models. Furthermore, the behavioral intention construct contained the highest scoring items in the ASCI survey (Table 1), as would be expected from a low-involvement hierarchy of attitude effect (Calder, 1979).
Evidence can be sought to support our assertions of the causal relationships between latent constructs in our students’ data, but it is very difficult to prove causality without a priori considerations built in to the survey instrument, none of which were built in or are present in the ASCI instrument. Therefore, we cautiously proceed in stating that evidence from our analysis of our students’ attitude data, along with theoretical considerations, point to a low-involvement (CBA) hierarchy of attitude effect, while acknowledging that our assertions fall short of definitive proof of this hierarchy effect.
The work presented herein suggests that our students are making decisions based on what they know or believe they know about the subject of chemistry, then are considering future behavioral intentions. Finally, the cognitive and behavioral intention components of our students’ attitude structure are producing feelings (the affective component) and remain concomitants of our students’ feelings toward the subject of chemistry.
If accurate, these preliminary low-involvement hierarchy (CBA) results suggest that our students are not as invested as they could be in making decisions about introductory chemistry, as evidenced by the removal of behavioral intention as the culminating act of the attitude structure and its replacement with the affective component. If true, and not just a reflection of the survey results, this hierarchy of attitude effect finding provides an opportunity to develop metacognitive and attitudinal interventions to address attitude mindsets at the beginning of an introductory chemistry course.
This work suggests that the original ASCI instrument is suited to the investigation of a hierarchy of attitude effect when attitude is the focus rather than its link to achievement. The hierarchy of attitude effect offers a potential lens towards understanding students’ attitude toward chemistry and monitoring academic interventions that might impact students’ attitude.
The results from this PLS-SEM analysis of students’ attitude toward the subject of chemistry chart a path for chemistry educators to be able to investigate the hierarchy of attitude effect being displayed within a given cohort in an institution and to pinpoint which aspects of students’ attitude are important. For example, faced with a low-involvement (CBA) hierarchy of attitude effect, the remedial goal should aim to reverse the hierarchical placement of behavioral intention and affect, hence transitioning from the low-involvement (CBA) to the high-involvement (CAB) hierarchy. It is possible that students’ attitude toward chemistry can be measured and redirected in a more positive direction within a semester if necessary (Abdullah et al., 2009; Koponen et al., 2012; Vishnumolakala et al., 2017). Engaging students in metacognitive discussions about different hierarchy of attitude effects would also likely be a useful component of remedial dialogue between students and instructors (Cook et al., 2013). For example, instructors could engage students with a short classroom presentation on the hierarchy of attitude effects, including what they are exactly and what they say about learning in the classroom. Time could then be made available to students to discuss the implications of each hierarchy toward learning chemistry. It would be important to allow students to think about the hierarchy of attitude effects and their confluence with academic performance.
Sole use of the shortened ASCIv2 instrument eliminates the possibility of capturing the hierarchy of attitude effect since ASCIv2 has the capacity to monitor only two of the three constituents of attitude (Xu and Lewis, 2011). Measurement of all three constituents of attitude are required to evaluate the hierarchy of attitude effect. While behavioral intention was the most positive component of attitude with our students, structural data presented herein showed that affect was the component of attitude most predicted by the other two latent constructs (Fig. 1 and 2). We believe that our students’ data was best modeled by the low-involvement (CBA) hierarchy of attitude effect (model 7). Together with previous studies, this work answers a call in the literature to further demonstrate that the ASCI instrument is a robust and valuable tool that can provide tailored insights into the attitudes of a diverse student cohort that might be used to augment the meaningful learning of chemistry.
Following on from this work, we plan to use PLS-SEM analysis of the ASCI instrument to evaluate the impact of learning interventions on the hierarchy of attitude effects in future cohorts of introductory chemistry students in our institution. A shift in students’ attitude from a low-involvement (CBA) hierarchy to a high-involvement (CAB) hierarchy of effect would be desirable and would signal a successful learning intervention. Another avenue for future work is to combine the results of students’ attitude toward chemistry with other affective domain markers (Bauer, 2005; Ferrell and Barbera, 2015). A vertical study of students’ affective domain during the transition from introductory chemistry to general chemistry to organic chemistry, and students’ sentiment toward electronic textbooks would also be beneficial to future planning and curriculum design in our institution and this work is currently underway.
Second, no structural model is perfect and so we have to accept and work with the best models available to us (MacCallum, 2003). While we have made every effort to present and discuss the best models of our data, grounded in sensible attitudinal theory and not just patterns in data, the structural models presented here are not perfect and therefore conclusions drawn from those models are likewise imperfect. For example, the tripartite structure of attitude does not exist in isolation and we have not considered extraneous interactions with our proposed hierarchy of attitude effect model (Breckler, 1984; Bhattacherjee and Sanford, 2006). Furthermore, this study adopted a PLS path analysis approach to investigate a possible hierarchy of attitude effect and, in doing so, forwent the chance to measure the structural model fit offered by CB-SEM. We encourage others with larger sample sizes to examine evidence of a hierarchy of attitude effect using a covariance-based approach. This is much easier to do with data from large 4 year institutions where the numbers are large enough to allow CB-SEM analysis.
Third, the participants in this study were mainly Hispanic/Latino students, which could limit the generalizability of the results to an institution with a different student make-up. Whereas we typically observe the Asian/Pacific Islander students in our chemistry classes outperform the majority Hispanic/Latino students, we did not investigate the structural model of each race group separately due to insufficient sample sizes needed to do so. Instead, our data incorporates attitudinal data from all students. We acknowledge that it is possible that the smaller sample of typically higher performing Asian/Pacific Islander students in our study could have a different structural model (or hierarchy effect) if analyzed in isolation. Furthermore, we did not account for the possibility that some students might be taking introductory chemistry for the first time or are repeating the class. Instead, we made a broad assumption that all students were encountering college-level introductory chemistry for the first time and were developing an attitude toward the subject of chemistry from a level starting point, and not from prior experience.
Fourth, this study was performed during an intensive 5 week Winter intersession rather than during a regular 15 week semester. The literature shows that students’ attitudes settle with time and exposure to an attitudinal object (Brandriet et al., 2013). Therefore, superior structural models can be forged from data surveyed later in a course. It cannot be assumed that an attitude toward the subject of chemistry surveyed at the end of a 5 week exposure would be identical to an attitude surveyed at the end of a 15 week exposure to introductory chemistry. Although we do not claim in this paper that results from a 5 week course can be extrapolated to a regular 15 week semester, we do caution against doing so here, and recommend that re-measuring students’ attitude during a full 15 week semester is necessary if a comparison is of interest.
Despite the aforementioned limitations, we believe that the work presented here will be of value to the broader chemistry education community. The link between students’ cognitive and affective domains during science classes, and the responsibility afforded to science educators to cater to both, is particularly relevant in today's science classrooms. PLS-SEM extends to chemistry educators with modest class sizes the opportunity to explore the unique affective domain of students within a cohort at any type of institution. PLS-SEM, coupled with the potential of the hierarchy of attitude effect to function as a lens through which to analyze the impact of instruction on students’ attitude, offers the possibility of monitoring the impact of learning interventions on an increasingly diverse student demographic.
Item number | Item | Week 1 (N = 98) | Week 5 (N = 85) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Skew | Kurt | Mean (SD) | Skew | Kurt | |||
a Negatively stated item is reversed before averaging. | ||||||||
1a | Easy | Hard | 3.388 (1.352) | 0.051 | 0.055 | 3.624 (1.543) | 0.021 | −0.615 |
2 | Worthless | Beneficial | 5.816 (1.409) | −1.538 | 2.341 | 5.682 (1.071) | −0.460 | −0.560 |
3a | Exciting | Boring | 4.571 (1.741) | −0.332 | −0.775 | 5.000 (1.447) | −0.265 | −0.535 |
4 | Complicated | Simple | 3.388 (1.469) | 0.355 | 0.014 | 3.129 (1.454) | 0.341 | −0.304 |
5 | Confusing | Clear | 3.878 (1.594) | 0.236 | −0.640 | 3.612 (1.423) | 0.161 | −0.000 |
6a | Good | Bad | 5.337 (1.631) | −0.867 | 0.085 | 5.224 (1.199) | 0.022 | −1.168 |
7a | Satisfying | Frustrating | 4.214 (1.508) | 0.068 | −0.495 | 4.529 (1.615) | −0.184 | −0.536 |
8a | Scary | Fun | 3.602 (1.470) | −0.016 | −0.264 | 3.671 (1.592) | 0.234 | −0.591 |
9a | Comprehensible | Incomprehensible | 4.571 (1.193) | −0.080 | 0.300 | 4.800 (1.317) | −0.326 | 0.333 |
10 | Challenging | Not challenging | 2.847 (1.515) | 0.882 | 0.476 | 2.718 (1.477) | 0.912 | 0.669 |
11a | Pleasant | Unpleasant | 4.490 (1.203) | −0.012 | 0.083 | 4.729 (1.409) | −0.627 | 0.716 |
12a | Interesting | Dull | 5.602 (1.504) | −1.109 | 0.668 | 5.612 (1.273) | −1.071 | 1.356 |
13a | Disgusting | Attractive | 3.082 (1.329) | 0.224 | 0.212 | 3.165 (1.174) | −0.102 | −0.522 |
14a | Comfortable | Uncomfortable | 4.235 (1.208) | 0.467 | 0.624 | 4.376 (1.431) | −0.070 | −0.104 |
15a | Worthwhile | Useless | 5.429 (1.392) | −0.949 | 0.913 | 5.118 (1.418) | −0.392 | −0.695 |
16a | Work | Play | 4.990 (1.576) | −0.579 | 0.110 | 4.965 (1.607) | −0.541 | −0.077 |
17 | Chaotic | Organized | 4.837 (1.337) | 0.094 | −0.644 | 4.729 (1.606) | −0.427 | −0.265 |
18 | Safe | Dangerous | 4.061 (1.442) | 0.038 | 0.110 | 3.871 (1.494) | −0.058 | −0.141 |
19a | Tense | Relaxed | 4.469 (1.408) | −0.320 | 0.001 | 4.412 (1.606) | −0.137 | −0.316 |
20a | Insecure | Secure | 3.663 (1.218) | −0.128 | 0.804 | 3.612 (1.372) | 0.029 | 0.090 |
Latent variable | Item number | Cronbach's alpha | Correlation | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Our data (N = 98) | Lit.a | Lit.b (N = 405) | Our data (N = 98) | Lit.a | Lit.b (N = 10) | ||
a Bauer, 2008. b Xu and Lewis, 2011. | |||||||
Interest & utility | (2,3,6,12,15) | 0.76 | 0.83 | 0.82 | 0.55 | 0.74 | 0.91 |
Anxiety | (8,13,16,19,20) | 0.72 | 0.77 | 0.71 | 0.68 | 0.64 | 0.96 |
Intellectual accessibility | (1,4,5,9,10) | 0.70 | 0.78 | 0.79 | 0.60 | 0.71 | 0.96 |
Emotional satisfaction | (7,11,14,17) | 0.78 | 0.79 | 0.74 | 0.72 | 0.72 | 0.96 |
Item | Our data (week 5) | Bauer (2008) | Xu and Lewis (2011) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F1 | F2 | F3 | F4 | F1 | F2 | F3 | F4 | F1 | F2 | F3 | F4 | |||
EFA was performed in XLSTAT 2017.5 using literature parameters, including principal components analysis with varimax rotation. Four factors were extracted for comparison with the literature. Expected item loadings are in bold. Item loadings onto unexpected factors are in italics.a Negatively stated. | ||||||||||||||
Interest and utility | ||||||||||||||
15a | Worthwhile | Useless | 0.83 | 0.16 | 0.01 | 0.26 | 0.85 | 0.01 | −0.06 | −0.11 | 0.75 | −0.22 | 0.01 | 0.06 |
2 | Worthless | Beneficial | 0.74 | −0.11 | −0.05 | −0.10 | −0.79 | −0.10 | 0.03 | −0.04 | −0.68 | 0.21 | 0.05 | 0.04 |
6a | Good | Bad | 0.82 | −0.16 | −0.12 | −0.04 | 0.71 | 0.05 | −0.20 | −0.04 | 0.68 | −0.20 | −0.16 | 0.13 |
12a | Interesting | Dull | 0.80 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 0.16 | 0.67 | 0.32 | 0.02 | −0.15 | 0.80 | −0.17 | −0.13 | −0.01 |
3a | Exciting | Boring | 0.82 | −0.12 | −0.00 | 0.11 | 0.58 | 0.38 | −0.05 | −0.09 | 0.69 | 0.08 | −0.24 | −0.08 |
Anxiety | ||||||||||||||
19a | Tense | Relaxed | −0.14 | 0.53 | −0.58 | −0.12 | −0.14 | −0.75 | 0.32 | 0.02 | −0.17 | 0.55 | 0.45 | 0.31 |
16a | Work | Play | 0.12 | 0.56 | −0.52 | 0.33 | 0.06 | −0.74 | 0.23 | −0.15 | −0.14 | 0.23 | 0.36 | 0.71 |
8a | Scary | Fun | −0.61 | 0.13 | −0.42 | −0.25 | −0.35 | −0.60 | 0.18 | 0.16 | −0.38 | 0.26 | 0.53 | −0.15 |
20a | Insecure | Secure | −0.24 | 0.74 | −0.07 | −0.39 | −0.34 | −0.53 | 0.23 | 0.29 | −0.20 | 0.64 | 0.37 | −0.17 |
13a | Disgusting | Attractive | −0.70 | 0.18 | 0.11 | −0.08 | −0.42 | −0.53 | −0.01 | 0.11 | −0.55 | 0.44 | 0.08 | 0.16 |
Intellectual accessibility | ||||||||||||||
4 | Complicated | Simple | −0.06 | −0.29 | 0.83 | −0.05 | −0.03 | −0.13 | 0.80 | −0.13 | −0.05 | 0.17 | 0.72 | 0.05 |
5 | Confusing | Clear | 0.17 | −0.30 | 0.73 | 0.03 | −0.24 | −0.33 | 0.75 | 0.06 | −0.10 | 0.04 | 0.78 | −0.06 |
1a | Easy | Hard | 0.27 | −0.04 | 0.77 | 0.21 | 0.13 | 0.18 | −0.73 | −0.34 | 0.19 | −0.24 | −0.70 | −0.07 |
10 | Challenging | Unchallenging | −0.17 | 0.15 | 0.83 | 0.10 | 0.29 | −0.36 | 0.54 | −0.01 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.69 | 0.36 |
9a | Comprehensible | Incomprehensible | 0.50 | −0.37 | 0.36 | 0.24 | 0.38 | −0.03 | −0.52 | −0.41 | 0.49 | −0.12 | −0.52 | 0.20 |
Fear | ||||||||||||||
18 | Safe | Dangerous | −0.06 | 0.15 | −0.07 | −0.88 | 0.03 | 0.05 | −0.05 | −0.85 | 0.09 | −0.29 | −0.18 | 0.68 |
Emotional satisfaction | ||||||||||||||
11a | Pleasant | Unpleasant | 0.70 | −0.14 | 0.32 | 0.19 | 0.50 | 0.44 | −0.35 | −0.27 | 0.60 | −0.13 | −0.50 | −0.10 |
14a | Comfortable | Uncomfortable | 0.48 | −0.14 | 0.27 | 0.49 | 0.48 | 0.43 | −0.35 | −0.28 | 0.46 | −0.42 | −0.48 | 0.03 |
17 | Chaotic | Unchaotic | 0.43 | −0.49 | 0.21 | 0.10 | −0.44 | −0.34 | 0.32 | −0.15 | −0.25 | 0.73 | 0.08 | −0.07 |
7a | Satisfying | Unsatisfying | 0.74 | −0.21 | 0.29 | 0.01 | 0.41 | 0.30 | −0.46 | −0.28 | 0.49 | −0.07 | −0.67 | 0.03 |
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2018 |