K. Christopher Smith*a and
Savannah Villarrealb
aDepartment of Chemistry, University of Texas-Pan American, 1201 W. University Drive, Edinburg, TX 78539-2999, USA. E-mail: kcsmith@utpa.edu
bScience Department, Michael E. Fossum Middle School, 7800 N. Ware Road, McAllen, TX 78504, USA
First published on 11th June 2015
In Smith and Villarreal's recently published article in this journal (2015), several types of general chemistry students' misconceptions concerning the concept of particle position during physical changes were identified. One of the misconceptions identified was that given a solid sample of a substance, some students did not think that a selected particle would move far from its original position throughout the reversible physical changes of melting and dissolving. In a recent comment on this work (Langbeheim, 2015), it was suggested that the nature of the data collection instruments, which showed a “…particle model of nine particles that are confined within a relatively small area” (Langbeheim, 2015), may have contributed to this misconception. Langbeheim argued that the illustrations may have caused students to consider the particles' movement to be restricted, which may have resulted in students responding that a selected particle would not move far from its original position. Additionally, in the comment, Langbeheim reported on molecular dynamics simulations which showed that indeed, based on the data collection instruments in Smith and Villarreal's work (2015), a selected particle would not move far from its original position over a short initial time period (shorter than the typical time period over which melting occurs).In developing our published study (Smith and Villarreal, 2015), we conducted pilot studies in which we tested and refined our data collection instruments. The original data collection instruments we used in our first pilot study are presented in Fig. 1 and 2.
These data collection instruments were refined to their final form in our published study (Smith and Villarreal, 2015) and addressed several points. One of these points was that given the large area available to the particles in the instruments in Fig. 1 and 2, we did not find it likely that the particles would end up in the 3 × 3 grid arrangement portrayed in the final representation of each instrument. As such, the dimensions of the borders surrounding the particles in the representations were reduced so that the 3 × 3 grid arrangement of particles in the final representation of each instrument would appear more likely.
As seen from Fig. 1 and 2, the particles in the representations have a much greater area available to them compared to the data collection instruments in the published work (Smith and Villarreal, 2015). This pilot study did not have human subjects research approval from the Institutional Review Board, so we will not report specific results here. However, the nature of the students' misconceptions and the trends in the results of this pilot study were similar to the results reported in the published work. As such, we stand by the soundness of the interpretation of our results in the published work. We do, however, appreciate the value of the comment (Langbeheim, 2015) as it prompts further investigation of how factors such as consideration of time and the nature of the representations in the instruments might affect students' conceptions and responses.
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2015 |