Byron MC Michael Kadum and
Mageswary Karpudewan*
School of Educational Studies, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Malaysia. E-mail: kmageswary@usm.my
First published on 25th March 2025
This study explores the relationships among self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation in chemistry, focusing on the moderating roles of gender and locality among Malaysian pre-university students. Using a quantitative approach, data were collected from 556 students and analysed through partial least square-structural equation modelling (PLS-SEM). The findings indicate that both self-efficacy and metacognition significantly predict academic motivation. Specifically, metacognition had a stronger influence on academic motivation (β = 0.412) than self-efficacy (β = 0.288). Gender significantly moderated the relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation, with female students showing a stronger link between self-efficacy and motivation than male students (β = −0.07). However, locality did not significantly moderate the relationships between self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation. The combination of self-efficacy and metacognition accounted for 42.3% of the variance in academic motivation (R2 = 0.423). These findings provide valuable insights into the factors that drive academic motivation in chemistry education. They suggest that educators emphasise self-efficacy, especially among female students, and integrate metacognitive strategies into the curriculum to enhance pre-university student motivation. Future research should explore the effects of educational interventions targeting self-efficacy and metacognition on academic motivation in chemistry.
Self-efficacy and metacognition predominate among the various factors influencing academic motivation in chemistry. Higher levels of self-efficacy among students are associated with increased motivation to participate in academic pursuits, persevere through challenges, and set ambitious goals for their academic endeavours (Syskowski and Kunina-Habenicht, 2023). Metacognition, the awareness and regulation of one's cognitive processes, influences how students plan, monitor, and evaluate their learning strategies and motivates them to learn chemistry as they are better at managing challenges, preventing burnout, and experiencing a state of flow in their chemistry learning (Guo et al., 2022). These claims (i.e., self-efficacy and metacognition influence academic motivation) are particularly true for pre-university students (Zhou et al., 2020). Pre-university programmes are conducted to equip students with the necessary skills and knowledge to transition to university higher education successfully (Allen et al., 2022). Students enrolled in these programmes face greater academic demands than in secondary school. They are also expected to take more responsibility for their learning.
Beyond the effects of self-efficacy and metacognition on academic motivation, gender also influences motivation to learn chemistry (Salta and Koulougliotis, 2020; Ardura et al., 2021), self-efficacy (Sunny et al., 2017), and metacognition (Abdelrahman, 2020). While the effects of gender on these factors are well-documented individually, there is a scarcity of research on how gender collectively moderates the relationships between self-efficacy and academic motivation, as well as between metacognition and academic motivation. Similarly, locality, defined as rural or urban, also influences academic motivation (Saw and Agger, 2021), self-efficacy (Astalini et al., 2020), and metacognition (Bakkaloglu, 2020). The effects of locality on these factors are usually documented in isolation.
Strong evidence on the influence of metacognition and self-efficacy on academic motivation, as well as the moderating role of gender and locality in these relationships, has been explicitly presented in the aforementioned studies. However, a common limitation in these studies is that the influence of each variable is examined in isolation. In reality, these variables do not exist independently—students' metacognition and self-efficacy develop simultaneously, and they also belong to different gender groups and localities. While there is extensive literature in chemistry education analysing these variables separately, research exploring their complex interconnections, including moderating effects that reflect real-world settings, remains limited. This study aims to bridge this gap by collectively examining the effects of self-efficacy and metacognition on chemistry academic motivation among pre-university students, while also investigating how gender and locality moderate these relationships.
On the other hand, extrinsic motivation includes the desire to engage in chemistry class activities for external rewards or outcomes, such as grades, praise, or recognition (Meydan, 2021; Pratt et al., 2023). Elford et al. (2022) state that while extrinsic motivation can encourage students to complete tasks and achieve scholastic targets, it may not lead to a deep understanding or long-term interest in chemistry. This is because extrinsic motivation focuses on external incentives or pressures rather than genuine interest or personal satisfaction. Nevertheless, according to self-determination theory, extrinsic motivation ranges along a continuum from integrated regulation to external regulation, with the former being closely aligned with intrinsic motivation as it is more self-determined (Ryan and Deci, 2020). Apart from intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, Ryan and Deci (2020) assert that amotivation must be considered in understanding human motivation fully. Amotivation refers to a state in which individuals lack the motivation to act due to a perceived absence of competence or control over the outcomes of their actions. Unlike intrinsic motivation, which arises from genuine interest, or extrinsic motivation, driven by external rewards, amotivated individuals experience a disconnect between their efforts and potential results. In an educational setting, students may experience amotivation when they feel that their actions will not lead to success, resulting in disengagement and a lack of persistence in their academic pursuits.
Thus, Bandura's (1986) definition of self-efficacy is applied in this study as it highlights the key factors – mastery experiences, vicarious experiences, verbal persuasion, and emotional/physiological states – essential for successful engagement in chemistry learning. Mastery experiences are the most powerful influence on students' self-efficacy in chemistry, stemming from their direct successes or failures in tasks such as solving chemical equations, conducting experiments, or mastering complex concepts. Success in these tasks strengthens their self-efficacy, while repeated failures can diminish it. Vicarious experiences occur when students observe their peers successfully completing chemistry-related tasks, such as performing lab experiments or explaining complex theories. This can enhance their belief in their abilities through modeling. Verbal persuasion, including encouragement and feedback from teachers, lab partners, and classmates, boosts students’ confidence in tackling chemistry challenges. Finally, students’ self-efficacy in chemistry is influenced by their emotional and physiological states, such as stress or anxiety during exams or lab work. Effectively managing these responses can enhance their confidence and engagement in chemistry learning.
Several studies have identified self-efficacy as a key factor in academic motivation. Abdolrezapour and colleagues (2023) observed a positive relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation in the context of online education. Additionally, Shofiah et al. (2023) found that, among Indonesian undergraduates, self-efficacy mediated the relationship between academic motivation and achievement during online learning. While the broader academic literature provides substantial evidence of the link between self-efficacy and academic motivation, this connection has not been extensively explored in chemistry education settings. However, illuminating insights emerge from a study conducted by Alci (2015) involving general chemistry undergraduates. Alci's (2015) data analysis revealed significant positive correlations between self-efficacy and extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. A correlation between self-efficacy and intrinsic and extrinsic motivations is also evident in studies conducted using the Science Motivation Questionnaire II (Glynn et al., 2011) and the Chemistry Motivation Questionnaire II (Salta and Koulougliotis, 2015). Another study involving pre-university students in a different US institution highlighted that self-efficacy is interconnected with other motivational constructs, such as personal and situational interest (Ferrell et al., 2016). Therefore, this suggests hypothesis H1: there is a significant positive effect of self-efficacy on academic motivation in chemistry among pre-university students.
Metacognition and academic motivation exhibit a positive correlation. Educational activities focusing on metacognition are linked to increased student motivation (Saxena, 2020). The study conducted in Canada with high school students revealed that they exhibited a higher level of metacognition in their learning processes, establishing a noteworthy positive correlation with their academic motivation (Landine and Stewart, 1998). Similarly, Abdelrahman (2020) conducted research in the United Arab Emirates (UAE) among undergraduates, emphasising a significant and positive association between students' metacognition and academic motivation. Furthermore, the findings underscored robust correlations: one between metacognitive knowledge awareness and academic intrinsic motivation and another between metacognitive regulation awareness and intrinsic motivation (Abdelrahman, 2020). Sen's (2016) study explored the impact of metacognitive levels on the motivation of Turkish chemistry preservice teachers. The investigation delved into the correlation between metacognition and academic motivation among the participants. Results revealed a significant difference in the mean intrinsic motivation score among high-intermediate and high-low groups, with no notable distinction in the intermediate-low category (Sen, 2016). The claims above lead to hypothesis H2: there is a significant positive effect of metacognition on academic motivation in chemistry among pre-university students.
The research on how gender influences chemistry self-efficacy presents mixed findings. Moreno and colleagues (2021) report that all undergraduate groups experienced increased chemistry self-efficacy after a semester, with both male and female students showing similar growth over time. However, another study conducted among 300 chemistry pre-service teachers in Indonesia found that female participants were significantly more self-efficacious than their male counterparts (Wahyudiati et al., 2019). Similarly, a study by Whitcomb et al. (2020) reported that female engineering students outperformed male participants in chemistry courses, and their self-efficacy aligns with their performance. However, over time, a non-significant gender gap in chemistry self-efficacy emerged, with male engineering students having slightly higher self-efficacy in several areas (Whitcomb et al., 2020). This echoes a study that found male undergraduates at a US institution believed more strongly in their ability to succeed in chemistry than their female peers, which led to higher test anxiety among female undergraduates (Sunny et al., 2017). Thus, these findings indicate gender differences in chemistry self-efficacy (Sunny et al., 2017; Wahyudiati et al., 2019; Whitcomb et al., 2020). The literature points to factors contributing to gender differences in chemistry self-efficacy, such as self-regulatory strategies (Seyhan, 2016) and anxiety and psychological factors (Amaliyah et al., 2021). The effects of gender on academic motivation and self-efficacy, as explained earlier, explain the formulation of hypothesis H3: gender moderates the relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation.
Research on the influence of gender on metacognition in chemistry is limited, and the findings that do exist present mixed results. Jumani et al. (2010) studied 900 students in Pakistan, and the findings indicated similar metacognitive awareness levels between males and females. This aligns with a recent study by Ahmed and colleagues (2019) involving 125 Indonesian high school students. The male and female students were equally competent in the three (3) metacognitive knowledge domains, i.e., declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge, and conditional knowledge, specifically in the chemical bonding topic (Ahmed et al., 2019). Therefore, these studies indicate that metacognition levels in chemistry are not affected by gender, especially in the context of high school students. However, a study conducted among undergraduates revealed differences in metacognition between males and females. A qualitative study reported by Muteti et al. (2023) showed that female general chemistry students at one institution in the United States used metacognitive strategies less than males before a 50-metacognition instruction, but the metacognitive equity gap was reduced over the semester of data collection following the metacognition lesson. Similarly, in a related science field (i.e., biology), research revealed that female university undergraduates in Indonesia might be more inclined toward metacognition than their male counterparts, potentially contributing to females demonstrating superior problem-solving skills in science (Adiansyah et al., 2021). Merchán Garzón et al. (2020) suggest that the factors contributing to the differences in metacognition levels between males and females may include social, educational, or psychological factors that influence how both genders approach learning and problem-solving in chemistry. The effects of gender on academic motivation and metacognition, as explained earlier, explain the formulation of hypothesis H4: gender moderates the relationship between metacognition and academic motivation.
There is scant research on the influence of geographical location (e.g., urban and rural) on students’ chemistry self-efficacy. Nevertheless, the available study's results suggest no significant difference between the urban and rural groups. For instance, a study in Ethiopia reported that rural secondary school students were slightly more self-efficacious in chemistry, but the differences between urban and rural students were not statistically significant (Asfaw, 2022). Findings from general science indicate that locality may play a significant role in determining students’ self-efficacy levels. For example, a study involving 926 junior high schools (i.e., 511 urban students and 415 rural students) in Indonesia revealed that urban students tend to have higher confidence in learning natural sciences than their rural counterparts (Astalini et al., 2020). Similarly, Ibrahim et al. (2019) studied 16-year-old Malaysian students from two (2) urban and two (2) rural secondary schools and found that urban students exhibited more favourable attitudes toward learning physics compared to rural students, which could imply higher self-efficacy. This disparity is attributed to urban students' greater exposure to diverse experiences and role models in science (Hill et al., 2018) and access to more effective pedagogical approaches (Bolshakova et al., 2011). The effects of locality on academic motivation and self-efficacy, as explained earlier, explain the formulation of hypothesis H5: locality moderates the relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation.
Research on how urban and rural localities influence students’ metacognition in the context of chemistry education is limited. However, evidence suggests that urban students are generally more likely to utilise metacognition than their rural counterparts (Taghieh et al., 2019). For instance, a study conducted in Turkey found that urban primary and secondary school students had significantly higher levels of metacognitive awareness than their rural peers. This difference was attributed to a more active educational environment and greater exposure to stimuli in urban schools (Bakkaloglu, 2020). Studies in fields related to chemistry also provide valuable insights into how geographical location may influence metacognition, particularly in science settings. For example, Alam (2020) examined 840 secondary school students studying biology in India. The findings indicated that urban students demonstrated greater competence in applying metacognitive strategies than their rural peers, contributing to better biology performance among urban students. According to Alam (2020), this difference in metacognition levels may be due to urban students' exposure to better educational environments. Interestingly, a study of metacognition levels in physics among Thai high school students revealed that urban and rural classrooms lacked sufficient metacognitive practices (Pimvichai et al., 2015). Despite urban students having slightly better access to environments conducive to metacognitive development, the impact of geographical location on metacognition was nuanced and complex (Pimvichai et al., 2015). This suggests that the influence of geographical location on students’ metacognition may vary depending on the specific context and subject matter. The effects of locality on academic motivation and metacognition, as explained earlier, explain the formulation of hypothesis H6: locality moderates the relationship between metacognition and academic motivation.
In Malaysia, several studies have examined self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation, particularly within the broader field of science education (Leong et al., 2018; Choy et al., 2020; Wong et al., 2021). However, despite these efforts, similar to the global scenario, the integration of these variables, specifically within chemistry education, remains limited. The existing literature has predominantly focused on general science disciplines, while the unique cognitive demands of chemistry, a core subject in pre-university programmes, are often overlooked. Moreover, while studies have investigated the role of location (rural vs. urban) on academic outcomes for science students, few have explored the specific impacts on chemistry teaching and learning. The same can be said for gender, where most research has centred on general academic performance or participation in science subjects, leaving a gap in understanding how gender moderates explicitly the relationship between self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation in chemistry education. The six hypotheses presented in this study aim to address the gap in the existing literature regarding these factors. This is crucial for designing chemistry-specific content and targeted teaching strategies that cater to the diverse needs of students. Examining these under-researched influences contributes to a deeper understanding of the cognitive and demographic factors affecting academic motivation in chemistry education, ultimately supporting the development of more effective pedagogical approaches (Graham et al., 2019).
Fig. 1 illustrates this study's conceptual framework. It depicts the hypothesised relationships among self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation in chemistry, with gender and locality as moderating variables.
![]() | ||
Fig. 1 Conceptual framework of the influence of self-efficacy and metacognition on academic motivation with gender and locality as moderators. |
(a) H1: self-efficacy significantly positively affects academic motivation in chemistry among pre-university students.
(b) H2: metacognition significantly positively affects academic motivation in chemistry among pre-university students.
(c) H3: gender significantly moderates the effect between self-efficacy and chemistry academic motivation among pre-university students.
(d) H4: gender significantly moderates the effect between metacognition and chemistry academic motivation among pre-university students.
(e) H5: locality significantly moderates the effect between self-efficacy and chemistry academic motivation among pre-university students.
(f) H6: locality significantly moderates the effect between metacognition and chemistry academic motivation among pre-university students.
The study was conducted at a government-run pre-university institution in Malaysia that delivers science courses comparable to the Cambridge Advanced Level (A-Level). Typically, students admitted to this institution are high achievers in the Sijil Pelajaran Malaysia (SPM) examination, equivalent to the British General Certificate of Education Ordinary Level (GCE O-Level). After completing their pre-university studies, these students commonly pursue further undergraduate education locally and at international universities, focusing on diverse scientific disciplines. The pre-university institution offers a variety of science-focused courses, including chemistry, biology, physics, computer science, and mathematics. The chemistry course has three main components: physical, inorganic, and organic. Students study fundamental concepts such as thermodynamics, kinetics, bonding, and reaction mechanisms, which provide a solid foundation for future studies in engineering and medicine. The course structure combines theoretical lectures with practical laboratory sessions, allowing students to apply scientific concepts through hands-on experiments. Assessments are conducted through continuous coursework, practical reports, and final examinations, ensuring a balanced evaluation of theoretical understanding and practical skills. Furthermore, the course promotes active learning through group discussions and peer-led activities. The chemistry curriculum is delivered over two semesters and organised under a modular system, which ensures a balanced workload and comprehensive coverage of key topics.
A minimum sample size of 178 was suggested based on the results of the G*Power 3.1.9.7 calculation: F tests family; linear multiple regression, fixed model, R2 deviation from zero; A priori, 0.95 level of power, 0.15 effect size (f2) at significance level p < 0.05, and eleven (11) predictors (3 CCSS factors, 6 CMI factors, gender, and locality). Therefore, the sample size acquired for this study (n = 556) was deemed sufficient. The pre-university institution was located using a convenience sampling strategy based on accessibility and willingness to participate. The institution represents typical Malaysian pre-university institutions offering comparable science-focused curricula. This is due to its alignment with national academic standards, the high academic achievement of its students, and the variety of science courses it offers, including chemistry, which mirrors the offerings in other pre-university programmes nationwide. Therefore, performing the research in an institution chosen based on convenience does not limit the generalisation of the findings to other pre-university institutions.
Malaysia has 15 government-run pre-university colleges, each enrolling approximately 1500 to 3000 students per intake. The pre-university institution where this study was conducted typically enrols between 1900 and 2400 science students per cohort. Among the 556 students who participated, the gender distribution included 150 males (27%) and 406 females (73%). Of these students, 301 (54%) came from rural areas, while 255 (46%) were from urban backgrounds. For comparison, the science student population at this pre-university institution during the study comprised 2285 students, with a gender distribution of approximately 30% males and 70% females, closely mirroring our sample's gender ratio. Similarly, the science student population's rural-to-urban ratio was about 60:
40, comparable to this study's sample's rural and urban composition. This study defines urban students as those whose hometowns and schooling before the pre-university programme were in urban areas, with access to more comprehensive educational resources and facilities to support learning in science subjects. In contrast, rural students are those from rural hometowns, attending primary and secondary schools in areas with more limited educational resources and infrastructure. This classification is crucial in examining how locality influences students' academic experiences and outcomes within the pre-university college setting.
The adaptation of Bandura's (1986) concept of self-efficacy into instruments developed by Dalgety et al. (2003) and Uzuntiryaki and Yeşim Çapa Aydın (2009) demonstrates the evolving application of self-efficacy theory within chemistry education. Dalgety et al. (2003) designed the Chemistry Attitudes and Experiences Questionnaire (CAEQ) to assess chemistry self-efficacy, focusing on students' confidence in performing task-specific academic skills, such as learning and applying chemistry theory. This aligns with Bandura's (1986) definition of self-efficacy as the judgment of one's ability to organise and execute tasks. However, their scale primarily emphasised cognitive aspects and did not encompass practical laboratory skills like handling equipment, materials, and chemicals in a laboratory setting. Uzuntiryaki and Çapa Aydın (2009) expanded on this by developing the College Chemistry Self-Efficacy Scale (CCSS), which broadened self-efficacy measurement to include three dimensions: cognitive skills, psychomotor skills (e.g., laboratory tasks), and the everyday application of chemistry. This broader approach built upon Dalgety et al.'s (2003) work by integrating both theoretical and practical components of self-efficacy, creating a more comprehensive tool while maintaining Bandura's (1986) focus on task-specificity across various contexts. The second instrument of the current study, the CCSS (Uzuntiryaki and Çapa Aydın, 2009), is used to determine participants’ chemistry self-efficacy. This scale consists of 21 items rated on a five-point scale, from 1 (very poorly) to 5 (very well).
The third psychometric tool employed in this study was the Chemistry Metacognition Inventory (CMI). A modified version of Taasoobshirazi et al.'s (2015) Physics Metacognition Inventory (PMI). Building on Flavell's framework, Taasoobshirazi et al. (2015) extended this conceptualisation in their studies by distinguishing between knowledge of cognition and regulation of cognition in problem-solving. They identified declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge as critical for solving complex scientific problems, such as those in physics and chemistry, while highlighting the role of planning, monitoring, and evaluation in regulating learning during problem-solving. This integration of Flavell's (1979) holistic view of metacognition into educational contexts underscores the importance of understanding and managing cognition for successful scientific problem-solving. Taasoobshirazi and Farley (2013) recommended two (2) adjustments for researchers in chemistry education who wished to use the PMI. Firstly, they suggested substituting the word “physics” with “chemistry.” Secondly, they proposed revising the information management items to specify “molecular diagrams” instead of “free-body diagrams.” Following Taasoobshirazi and Farley's recommendation, the term “molecular diagram” was used. In chemistry education, molecular diagrams serve as visualisation tools to support understanding of molecular structures and the behaviour of atoms and molecules at the sub-microscopic level (Chittleborough and Treagust, 2008). The CMI psychometric tool includes 26 items rated on a five-point scale, extending from 1 (never true of myself) to 5 (always true of myself).
Science courses (physics, chemistry, and biology) at the pre-university level in Malaysia are taught in English, requiring students to possess considerable English language proficiency. The participating students demonstrated sufficient English proficiency to comprehend the items in the instruments, as reported in the pilot study.
Psychometric instrument | Cronbach's alpha |
---|---|
1. Academic motivation scale-chemistry (AMS-chemistry) | |
a. Factor A (AM-FA): amotivation | 0.74 |
b. Factor B (AM-FB): external regulation | 0.95 |
c. Factor C (AM-FC): introjected regulation | 0.89 |
d. Factor D (AM-FD): identified regulation | 0.95 |
e. Factor E (AM-FE): to experience, to accomplish, and to know | 0.94 |
2. College chemistry self-efficacy scale (CCSS) | |
a. Factor A (SE-FA): self-efficacy for cognitive skills | 0.93 |
b. Factor B (SE-FB): self-efficacy for psychomotor skills | 0.93 |
c. Factor C (SE-FC): self-efficacy for everyday applications | 0.86 |
3. Chemistry metacognition inventory (CMI) | |
a. Factor A (MC-FA): knowledge of cognition (declarative, procedural, and conditional) | 0.92 |
b. Factor B (MC-FB): regulation of cognition (information management – chemistry diagrams) | 0.94 |
c. Factor C (MC-FC): regulation of cognition (monitoring) | 0.90 |
d. Factor D (MC-FD): regulation of cognition (evaluation) | 0.95 |
e. Factor E (MC-FE): regulation of cognition (debugging) | 0.81 |
f. Factor F (MC-FF): regulation of cognition (planning) | 0.77 |
PLS analysis involved two models: the measurement and structural models (Hair et al., 2017). The measurement model, which reflects the relationship between latent and observed variables, was tested through reliability and validity analyses. Latent variables are theoretical constructs that cannot be directly measured but are inferred from relationships among multiple observed variables, representing underlying traits, attitudes, or characteristics in a model (Hair et al., 2019). These latent variables are indirectly measured through observed variables, which are directly measurable data points reflecting the presence or extent of these constructs (Hair et al., 2019). In this study, the three (3) chemistry-related latent variables (i.e., constructs) are academic motivation (AM), self-efficacy (SE), and metacognition (MC). The observed chemistry-related variables (i.e., items) include academic motivation (measured as AM1 to AM28), self-efficacy (measured as SE1 to SE21), and metacognition (measured as MC1 to MC26).
On the other hand, the structural model tests the hypothesised relationships between the latent variables and incorporates the role of moderators, such as gender and locality, in these relationships. The structural model evaluates how the constructs influence one another, quantified by path coefficients, effect sizes, R2, and Q2 values (Hair et al., 2017). Specifically, the structural model in this study assesses whether self-efficacy (SE) and metacognition (MC) influence academic motivation (AM). Additionally, it explores whether gender and locality moderate the strength and direction of these relationships. This analysis helps to determine if the impact of self-efficacy (SE) or metacognition (MC) on academic motivation (AM) differs between male and female students or between students from rural and urban areas.
Constructs | Items | λ | Convergent validity and reliability | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
rho_A | CR | AVE | |||
Academic motivation Factor A (AM-FA): amotivation | AM1 | 0.886 | 0.883 | 0.907 | 0.712 |
AM2 | 0.709 | ||||
AM3 | 0.869 | ||||
AM4 | 0.897 | ||||
Academic motivation Factor B (AM-FB): external regulation | AM5 | 0.858 | 0.960 | 0.959 | 0.855 |
AM6 | 0.941 | ||||
AM7 | 0.954 | ||||
AM8 | 0.943 | ||||
Academic motivation Factor C (AM-FC): introjected regulation | AM9 | 0.856 | 0.886 | 0.903 | 0.699 |
AM10 | 0.841 | ||||
AM11 | 0.765 | ||||
AM12 | 0.878 | ||||
Academic motivation Factor D (AM-FD): identified regulation | AM13 | 0.907 | 0.942 | 0.959 | 0.852 |
AM14 | 0.927 | ||||
AM15 | 0.921 | ||||
AM16 | 0.938 | ||||
Academic motivation Factor E (AM-FE): to experience, to accomplish, and to know | AM17 | 0.757 | 0.959 | 0.963 | 0.687 |
AM18 | 0.765 | ||||
AM19 | 0.765 | ||||
AM20 | 0.825 | ||||
AM21 | 0.844 | ||||
AM22 | 0.852 | ||||
AM23 | 0.840 | ||||
AM24 | 0.844 | ||||
AM25 | 0.851 | ||||
AM26 | 0.874 | ||||
AM27 | 0.875 | ||||
AM28 | 0.842 | ||||
Self-efficacy Factor A (SE-FA): self-efficacy for cognitive skills | SE1 | 0.773 | 0.930 | 0.940 | 0.612 |
SE2 | 0.794 | ||||
SE3 | 0.758 | ||||
SE4 | 0.759 | ||||
SE5 | 0.789 | ||||
SE6 | 0.796 | ||||
SE7 | 0.799 | ||||
SE8 | 0.801 | ||||
SE9 | 0.759 | ||||
SE12 | 0.791 | ||||
Self-efficacy Factor B (SE-FB): self-efficacy for psychomotor skills | SE10 | 0.807 | 0.927 | 0.941 | 0.696 |
SE11 | 0.742 | ||||
SE13 | 0.821 | ||||
SE14 | 0.875 | ||||
SE15 | 0.856 | ||||
SE16 | 0.862 | ||||
SE17 | 0.870 | ||||
Self-efficacy Factor C (SE-FC): self-efficacy for everyday applications | SE18 | 0.815 | 0.882 | 0.919 | 0.738 |
SE19 | 0.839 | ||||
SE20 | 0.899 | ||||
SE21 | 0.882 | ||||
Metacognition Factor A (MC-FA): knowledge of cognition (declarative, procedural, and conditional) | MC1 | 0.840 | 0.908 | 0.927 | 0.679 |
MC2 | 0.837 | ||||
MC3 | 0.827 | ||||
MC4 | 0.818 | ||||
MC5 | 0.838 | ||||
MC6 | 0.783 | ||||
Metacognition Factor B (MC-FB): regulation of cognition (information management – chemistry diagrams) | MC7 | 0.883 | 0.913 | 0.939 | 0.795 |
MC8 | 0.922 | ||||
MC9 | 0.925 | ||||
MC10 | 0.833 | ||||
Metacognition Factor C (MC-FC): regulation of cognition (monitoring) | MC11 | 0.854 | 0.907 | 0.934 | 0.779 |
MC12 | 0.868 | ||||
MC13 | 0.892 | ||||
MC14 | 0.915 | ||||
Metacognition Factor D (MC-FD): regulation of cognition (evaluation) | MC15 | 0.863 | 0.909 | 0.933 | 0.777 |
MC16 | 0.898 | ||||
MC17 | 0.902 | ||||
MC18 | 0.860 | ||||
Metacognition Factor E (MC-FE): regulation of cognition (debugging) | MC19 | 0.900 | 0.827 | 0.897 | 0.744 |
MC20 | 0.901 | ||||
MC21 | 0.782 | ||||
Metacognition Factor F (MC-FF): regulation of cognition (planning) | MC22 | 0.804 | 0.846 | 0.882 | 0.600 |
MC23 | 0.814 | ||||
MC24 | 0.703 | ||||
MC25 | 0.728 | ||||
MC26 | 0.817 |
The analysis of convergent validity and reliability for the constructs of academic motivation (AM), self-efficacy (SE), and metacognition (MC) demonstrates strong support for the measurement model. Hair et al. (2019) suggest that factor loadings (λ) exceeding 0.7 are good, implying that the items effectively capture the construct they are designed to measure. Based on the data in Table 2, all items for the current study's three (3) latent variables (i.e., constructs) fulfilled this requirement. Moreover, in Table 2, the composite reliability (CR) and Dijkstra-Henseler's rho_A values are similarly high and exceed the recommended threshold of 0.7 (Hair et al., 2020), further supporting the constructs' reliability. The average variance extracted (AVE) values for the factors range from 0.600 to 0.855, suggesting that their respective constructs explain a substantial proportion of variance in the items (Hair et al., 2020). These results affirm that the measurement scales effectively capture the intended constructs, providing a solid foundation for further analysis in this study.
The discriminant validity of the constructs in this study was assessed using the Heterotrait–Monotrait (HTMT) ratio of correlations, as shown in Table 3. HTMT values below 0.85 indicate adequate discriminant validity, signifying that the constructs are distinct (Henseler et al., 2015). The constructs in this analysis demonstrated acceptable discriminant validity, as all HTMT values fell well below the 0.85 threshold. For instance, the relationship between AM-FA and MC-FA yielded a value of 0.234, and between MC-FB and AM-FB, a value of 0.082. i.e., both well within acceptable limits. Thus, the constructs' discriminant validity has been confirmed for this study.
Factors | AM-FA | MC-FA | SE-FA | AM-FB | MC-FB | SE-FB | AM-FC | MC-FC | SE-FC | AM-FD | MC-FD | AM-FE | MC-FE | MC-FF |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AM-FA = amotivation; AM-FB = external regulation; AM-FC = introjected regulation; AM-FD = identified regulation; AM-FE = to experience, to accomplish, and to know; MC-FA = knowledge of cognition (declarative, procedural, and conditional); MC-FB = regulation of cognition (information management – chemistry diagrams); MC-FC = regulation of cognition (monitoring); MC-FD = regulation of cognition (evaluation); MC-FE = regulation of cognition (debugging); MC-FF = regulation of cognition (planning); SE-FA = self-efficacy for cognitive skills; SE-FB = self-efficacy for psychomotor skills; SE-FC = self-efficacy for everyday applications | ||||||||||||||
AM-FA | ||||||||||||||
MC-FA | 0.234 | |||||||||||||
SE-FA | 0.122 | 0.779 | ||||||||||||
AM-FB | 0.115 | 0.116 | 0.190 | |||||||||||
MC-FB | 0.074 | 0.575 | 0.501 | 0.082 | ||||||||||
SE-FB | 0.132 | 0.562 | 0.736 | 0.257 | 0.372 | |||||||||
AM-FC | 0.151 | 0.369 | 0.384 | 0.414 | 0.239 | 0.352 | ||||||||
MC-FC | 0.136 | 0.616 | 0.595 | 0.171 | 0.395 | 0.520 | 0.437 | |||||||
SE-FC | 0.040 | 0.663 | 0.807 | 0.253 | 0.479 | 0.666 | 0.337 | 0.522 | ||||||
AM-FD | 0.262 | 0.374 | 0.427 | 0.535 | 0.276 | 0.450 | 0.520 | 0.359 | 0.441 | |||||
MC-FD | 0.223 | 0.477 | 0.479 | 0.178 | 0.381 | 0.456 | 0.373 | 0.520 | 0.410 | 0.370 | ||||
AM-FE | 0.316 | 0.517 | 0.563 | 0.260 | 0.426 | 0.512 | 0.657 | 0.538 | 0.519 | 0.643 | 0.472 | |||
MC-FE | 0.215 | 0.419 | 0.404 | 0.202 | 0.382 | 0.454 | 0.358 | 0.517 | 0.386 | 0.421 | 0.602 | 0.452 | ||
MC-FF | 0.177 | 0.675 | 0.664 | 0.199 | 0.516 | 0.558 | 0.438 | 0.693 | 0.583 | 0.427 | 0.658 | 0.540 | 0.692 |
Factors | AM-FA | MC-FA | SE-FA | AM-FB | MC-FB | SE-FB | AM-FC | MC-FC | SE-FC | AM-FD | MC-FD | AM-FE | MC-FE | MC-FF |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AM-FA = amotivation; AM-FB = external regulation; AM-FC = introjected regulation; AM-FD = identified regulation; AM-FE = to experience, to accomplish, and to know; MC-FA = knowledge of cognition (declarative, procedural, and conditional); MC-FB = regulation of cognition (information management – chemistry diagrams); MC-FC = regulation of cognition (monitoring); MC-FD = regulation of cognition (evaluation); MC-FE = regulation of cognition (debugging); MC-FF = regulation of cognition (planning); SE-FA = self-efficacy for cognitive skills; SE-FB = self-efficacy for psychomotor skills; SE-FC = self-efficacy for everyday applications. | ||||||||||||||
AM-FA | ||||||||||||||
MC-FA | 2.512 | 2.512 | 2.512 | 2.512 | 2.512 | |||||||||
SE-FA | 3.534 | 3.534 | 3.534 | 3.534 | 3.534 | |||||||||
AM-FB | ||||||||||||||
MC-FB | 1.482 | 1.482 | 1.482 | 1.482 | 1.482 | |||||||||
SE-FB | 2.083 | 2.083 | 2.083 | 2.083 | 2.083 | |||||||||
AM-FC | ||||||||||||||
MC-FC | 1.869 | 1.869 | 1.869 | 1.869 | 1.869 | |||||||||
SE-FC | 2.333 | 2.333 | 2.333 | 2.333 | 2.333 | |||||||||
AM-FD | ||||||||||||||
MC-FD | 1.710 | 1.710 | 1.710 | 1.710 | 1.710 | |||||||||
AM-FE | ||||||||||||||
MC-FE | 1.686 | 1.686 | 1.686 | 1.686 | 1.686 | |||||||||
MC-FF | 2.471 | 2.471 | 2.471 | 2.471 | 2.471 |
The results of the structural model assessment are summarised in Tables 5–7, highlighting the significant relationships between the predictors (i.e., self-efficacy and metacognition) and academic motivation. Table 5 displays the path coefficients (i.e., direct hypothesis testing), where self-efficacy (β = 0.288) and metacognition (β = 0.412) strongly influence academic motivation in chemistry, with T statistics exceeding the threshold for significance and p-values of p < 0.001. Table 6 provides the metrics, revealing that the model accounts for approximately 42.3% of the variance in academic motivation, as evidenced by the R2 value of 0.423. The R2 value of above 0.260 indicates the model exhibits substantial level of predictive accuracy (Cohen, 1988). Effect sizes of f2 = 0.070 for self-efficacy and f2 = 0.142 metacognition demonstrates small and medium effect sizes of the predictive constructs on AM (Cohen, 1988). Finally, Table 7 presents the cross-validated redundancy analysis, which indicates a Q2 value of 0.179 for academic motivation, signifying the predictive relevance of the model (Hair et al., 2019). These results underscore the importance of self-efficacy and metacognition in enhancing academic motivation among Malaysian pre-university students, specifically in chemistry.
Hypotheses | β | M | SD | t | p | Decision |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β = path coefficient; M = sample mean; SD = standard deviation; t = T statistics; p = P Values. | ||||||
H1: self-efficacy → academic motivation | 0.288 | 0.285 | 0.063 | 4.564 | <0.001 | Supported |
H2: metacognition → academic motivation | 0.412 | 0.425 | 0.058 | 7.136 | <0.001 | Supported |
Metric | Value |
---|---|
R2 for academic motivation | 0.423 |
Adjusted R2 for academic motivation | 0.421 |
f2 (effect sizes): | |
H1: self-efficacy → academic motivation | 0.070 |
H2: metacognition → academic motivation | 0.142 |
Constructs | SSO | SSE | Q2 (=1-SSE/SSO) |
---|---|---|---|
SSO = sum of squared observations; SSE = sum of squared errors. | |||
Academic motivation | 2780 | 2282.816 | 0.179 |
Self-efficacy | 1668 | 1668 | |
Metacognition | 3336 | 3336 |
Variable | R2 Excluded | R2 Included | Difference |
---|---|---|---|
Self-efficacy | |||
Gender as moderator | 0.359 | 0.363 | 0.004 |
Locality as moderator | 0.352 | 0.354 | 0.002 |
Metacognition | |||
Gender as moderator | 0.387 | 0.388 | 0.001 |
Locality as moderator | 0.390 | 0.392 | 0.002 |
Hypotheses | β | M | SD | t | p | Decision |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β = path coefficient; M = sample mean; SD = standard deviation; t = T statistics; p = P values. | ||||||
H3: self-efficacy → academic motivation (moderated by gender) | −0.07 | −0.08 | 0.03 | 1.99 | 0.02 | Supported |
H4: metacognition → academic motivation (moderated by gender) | −0.03 | −0.05 | 0.05 | 0.51 | 0.30 | Rejected |
H5: self-efficacy → academic motivation (moderated by locality) | −0.05 | −0.03 | 0.06 | 0.78 | 0.22 | Rejected |
H6: metacognition → academic motivation (moderated by locality) | −0.05 | −0.02 | 0.08 | 0.62 | 0.27 | Rejected |
Hypothesis H2 is also supported, with metacognition having a significant and stronger positive impact on academic motivation (β = 0.412, p < 0.001) than self-efficacy. This finding aligns with existing research suggesting that metacognitive awareness and regulation promote academic motivation by enabling students to plan, monitor, and evaluate their learning effectively (Landine and Stewart, 1998; Abdelrahman, 2020). In Malaysia, metacognition's substantial impact on motivation may be particularly relevant due to the challenging content of pre-university chemistry (Matriculation Division, 2022a, b), which necessitates careful planning and self-monitoring. Malaysian students are often encouraged to take ownership of their learning, which can foster metacognitive skills (Choy et al., 2020). These skills allow students to navigate complex chemistry topics more confidently, preventing burnout and helping them achieve a sense of progress and engagement.
The results support hypothesis H3, showing that gender statistically significantly moderates the relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation (β = −0.07, p = 0.023). Female students in this study demonstrated a stronger link between self-efficacy and academic motivation than male students, consistent with findings in other STEM fields, such as those reported by Wang and Yu (2023). Interestingly, while higher self-efficacy among male students was associated with slightly lower academic motivation than females, this trend may reflect sociocultural influences unique to Malaysia. In Malaysia, societal expectations often encourage male students to project confidence and self-assurance (Saadat and Sultana, 2023); however, this outward confidence does not necessarily translate into greater motivation, particularly if they lack genuine engagement with the subject. Conversely, the pre-university educational environment in Malaysia, which includes supportive educators and challenging assignments, appears especially influential for female students. Opportunities for autonomy in learning and exposure to positive role models can further enhance their motivation and academic performance, highlighting the critical role of self-efficacy in fostering their academic engagement (Khalid and Rahman, 2023).
Hypothesis H4 is not supported, as gender did not significantly moderate the relationship between metacognition and academic motivation among Malaysian pre-university students (β = −0.03, p = 0.30) despite the study's expectations based on prior literature. This suggests that gender differences in motivation (Salta and Koulougliotis, 2020; Meydan, 2021; Ardura et al., 2023; Salsabila and Huda, 2023) and metacognition (Jumani et al., 2010; Ahmed et al., 2019; Adiansyah et al., 2021) exist independently. The findings do not support hypothesis H5. In contrast to the study's expectations informed by prior literature, locality did not significantly moderate the relationship between self-efficacy and academic motivation among pre-university chemistry students (β = −0.05, p = 0.22). Similarly, hypothesis H6 is not supported. Contrary to expectations and despite documented locality differences in academic motivation and metacognition, the findings reveal no significant moderation by locality on the relationship between metacognition and academic motivation in chemistry among Malaysian pre-university students (β = −0.05, p = 0.27). Although previous studies suggest that locality can independently influence self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation, with urban students often reporting higher self-efficacy (Ibrahim et al., 2019; Astalini et al., 2020), metacognition (Taghieh et al., 2019; Alam, 2020), and motivation (Saleh, 2014; Zhang and Zhou, 2023) due to better access to resources (Pimvichai et al., 2015; Hill et al., 2018; Hariyati et al., 2021), these effects did not extend to moderating the relationship between self-efficacy and metacognition with academic motivation in this study. The absence of moderating effects of locality on self-efficacy and metacognition's impact on academic motivation can be attributed to the homogenising influence of Malaysia's educational policies. The standardised pre-university chemistry curriculum (Matriculation Division, 2022a, b) and centralised STEM education policies (Ministry of Education Malaysia, 2024b) likely create an equalisation effect, minimising disparities. This environment promotes systematic study habits and strategic problem-solving approaches, reducing notable differences in academic motivation across localities and possibly genders.
In summary, this study confirms the significant roles of self-efficacy and metacognition in predicting academic motivation among Malaysian pre-university students, with metacognition having a more substantial influence. Gender significantly moderates the relationship between self-efficacy and motivation, highlighting the need for educational strategies that support both genders differently in STEM fields, especially chemistry. Although locality does not moderate the relationship between self-efficacy and motivation or between metacognition and motivation, the findings indicate that Malaysian pre-university students benefit from a relatively equalised educational experience. This is guided by the standardised pre-university curriculum and centralised STEM education policies. Educators should consider emphasising metacognitive strategies in the curriculum to enhance academic motivation, especially for challenging subjects like chemistry. Additionally, interventions to enhance self-efficacy may be particularly beneficial for female students, as this could further support their motivation in science-related subjects.
Theoretically, these results contribute to the ongoing discourse on academic motivation in chemistry education by highlighting the role of self-efficacy and metacognition as crucial predictors. The study reinforces existing theories suggesting that self-efficacy is a critical motivational driver, significantly when moderated by gender, adding nuance to our understanding of how male and female students engage with chemistry. This finding challenges traditional assumptions of gender-neutral self-efficacy models, suggesting that gender-specific interventions may be more effective.
Practically, the findings suggest that chemistry educators should focus on enhancing self-efficacy, particularly for female students, to foster stronger academic motivation. Additionally, integrating metacognitive strategies into the curriculum can benefit all students, providing tools to monitor and regulate their learning effectively. These insights could inform the design of gender-sensitive educational interventions and targeted teaching practices to improve motivation, confidence, and academic success in chemistry.
Despite its contributions, this study has limitations that may affect the generalisability of the findings. Firstly, focusing on a specific group of pre-university students limits applicability across broader educational contexts and diverse demographics. The relatively homogeneous sample and geographical scope may not capture varied educational experiences or motivational factors among different populations. Secondly, this study's cross-sectional design provides a snapshot rather than a longitudinal understanding of how self-efficacy and metacognition interact with academic motivation over time. Several methodological steps were taken to address these limitations and enhance the validity of the findings. The study administered well-validated chemistry psychometric instruments (i.e., AMS-Chemistry, CCSS, and CMI), ensuring reliable and accurate measurements across the target demographic. A pilot study involving a comparable pre-university student group was conducted to refine the instruments through cognitive interviews and expert reviews, confirming their relevance and applicability to Malaysian students. Finally, using PLS-SEM allowed for a detailed examination of the relationships among variables, including moderating effects, to ensure that findings accurately reflect the influences of self-efficacy and metacognition on academic motivation. The third limitation of this study is the relatively small sample size used for the CFA. Despite this, the CFA results indicate adequate model fit indices (MacCallum et al., 1996; Chen et al., 2018). Increasing the sample size would further enhance the robustness of the model fit.
Future research should address these limitations by including more diverse samples across educational levels and contexts to enhance generalisability. Longitudinal studies are also recommended to examine changes in the relationships among self-efficacy, metacognition, and academic motivation over time. Moreover, investigating the effects of educational interventions targeting self-efficacy and metacognition may provide insights into how these factors can be effectively enhanced to improve academic motivation in chemistry and other STEM fields. Such studies could provide valuable data on tailoring interventions to different demographic groups, further informing gender-sensitive and location-responsive educational practices.
In conclusion, this study offers valuable theoretical and practical insights into the key factors influencing academic motivation in chemistry, laying the groundwork for future research and educational strategies to improve student engagement and learning outcomes.
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2025 |