Megan C.
Connor
*,
Ally R.
Parvin
and
Alex F.
Browning
Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Samford University, Birmingham, Alabama 35229, USA. E-mail: mconnor@samford.edu
First published on 23rd December 2024
Women report that they must conform to masculine behavioral norms to progress in chemistry, with the necessity of adopting such norms pushing them from the field. Advancing gender-based equity within chemistry will thus entail identifying these norms, deconstructing them, and, ultimately, redefining them to be inclusive of all individuals. To support these efforts, this study investigates whether engaging in a traditional nuclear magnetic resonance (NMR) communication task versus a similar task with multiple identity-safe cues differentially impacts individuals’ gender stigma consciousness, or the extent to which individuals are acutely aware of stigma attached to their gender. Undergraduates (n = 543) enrolled in Organic Chemistry II at a large university in the southeastern United States completed an online NMR communication task followed by a version of the Social Identities and Attitudes Scale (SIAS) modified for use in chemistry learning environments (i.e., the SIAS-Chem). Participants were randomly assigned to one of two prompt groups prior to task completion: one group was told the task evaluates their NMR communication ability, and the other group was told the task was non-evaluative and used to understand the different ways people communicate. The results provide initial psychometric evidence of the SIAS-Chem's functionality and measurement invariance across prompt groups, providing preliminary support for its use in identifying chemistry practices that are potentially exclusionary of women. Further, women who were told the task evaluates NMR communication ability reported greater gender stigma consciousness on the SIAS-Chem compared to women who were told the task was non-evaluative, while there is no evidence of men scoring differently across prompts. Gender stigma consciousness was also associated with confidence during task completion among women who were told the task was non-evaluative. The findings have implications for the design of equitable assessments and instruction on NMR spectroscopy and future research on communication styles in chemistry.
Carlone and Johnson (2007) identify three subconstructs underlying science identity: competence, performance, and recognition. According to this framework, a woman with a strong science identity possesses a meaningful understanding of scientific content (i.e., competence). She is also able to demonstrate to others her competence with scientific practices (i.e., performance) through “uses of scientific tools, fluency with all forms of scientific talk and ways of acting, and interacting in various formal and informal scientific settings” (Carlone and Johnson, 2007). Lastly, she recognizes herself as a “science person” and others recognize her as a “science person” (i.e., recognition) through her demonstrated performance. This recognition is essential for developing a strong science identity (Carlone and Johnson, 2007).
Carlone and Johnson's (2007) science identity framework provides important theoretical grounding from which other identity frameworks have emerged (Hazari et al., 2010; Hosbein and Barbera, 2020a, 2020b). For instance, Hazari et al. (2010) also incorporate the subconstructs competence, performance, and recognition into their framework describing students’ physics identity. However, the competence and performance subconstructs are refined to focus on students’ belief in their ability to understand content and performed required tasks, respectively, rather than ability alone. Hazari et al. (2010) also identify interest as a fourth subconstruct underlying students’ physics identity, where they define interest as the desire or curiosity to think about and understand physics. Further, Hosbein and Barbera (2020a) developed a framework describing undergraduates’ chemistry identity, in which the subconstructs performance and competence are replaced with a new subconstruct termed mastery experiences. In this framework, mastery experiences constitute “experiences in which an individual is able to base their self-perception on successes and failures of specific tasks within a classroom” (Hosbein and Barbera, 2020a). The framework further breaks down Carlone and Johnson's (2007) recognition subconstruct into verbal persuasion (i.e., “verbal recognition for being good at… chemistry”) and vicarious experiences (i.e., “recognition of self through comparison of others’ experiences with… chemistry”) (Hosbein and Barbera, 2020a). Situational interest and mindset are then identified as additional factors underlying students’ chemistry identity. Situational interest is defined as a student's orientation to chemistry through “value- and feeling-related interest,” whereas mindset refers to the belief that participation in chemistry requires either inherent ability or determination (Hosbein and Barbera, 2020a).
When experiencing conflict between one's gender identity and identity as a scientist, many women perceive a need to negotiate their sense of self in order to be both a “woman” and a “scientist” (Archer et al., 2012; Danielsson, 2012). This is because certain science fields constitute masculine and heteronormative contexts where women who do not perform their gender in such ways are excluded (Danielsson, 2012; Calás et al., 2013; Avraamidou, 2020). This negotiation of self may involve redefining one's notion of what it means to be a “woman” or “scientist,” including the abandonment or change of mannerisms, behaviors, and the assumptions associated with them in order to form a cohesive sense of self (Archer et al., 2012; Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018). When considering the subconstructs identified in different science identity frameworks, such negotiations best relate to what Carlone and Johnson (2007) conceptualize as performance (e.g., ways of talking and acting). For instance, research demonstrates that women tend to abandon feminine behaviors (e.g., being “girly” and dressing fashionably) when they seek to create a unifying sense of self as a “woman” and a “scientist,” as they perceive such feminine behaviors to be incompatible with masculine behavioral norms in science (Archer et al., 2012). In particular, women in physics have been found to compromise their femininity to perform gender neutrality which, in turn, allows them to fit within the dominant culture of their department (Gonsalves, 2014). Through this abandonment of feminine behaviors, a woman would also be able to demonstrate her competence through “fluency with all forms of scientific talk and ways of acting” (Carlone and Johnson, 2007). This performance would then facilitate self-recognition and recognition by others as a “science person,” where such recognition is an essential aspect of forming a science identity that appears in all the aforementioned identity frameworks (Carlone and Johnson, 2007; Hazari et al., 2010; Hosbein and Barbera, 2020a).
A similar process of negotiation occurs among women seeking to form a chemistry identity (Grunert and Bodner, 2011; Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018). Chemistry graduate students who are women report a perceived need to change their feminine personalities and mannerisms and, in turn, make themselves more masculine if they wish to succeed in future chemistry research careers (Grunert and Bodner, 2011). They also report a need to acquire more masculine characteristics and mannerisms to adequately interact with their peers in a professional setting, specifically by appearing less emotional and more analytical during these interactions (Grunert and Bodner, 2011). Some of these women recognize a change in their own personalities during their time in graduate school, progressively becoming more “severe;” others indicate that they do not wish to change these aspects of their personality and that working in industry or at a research-intensive university would require them to do so (Grunert and Bodner, 2011).
Among women who are successful chemists, research has found that they effectively incorporate a masculine performance into their gender identity, allowing their professional identity to be predominately masculine while their personal identity could remain predominantly feminine (Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018). These women further indicate that there are ways to perform femininity (i.e., act like a woman) that contradict with acting like a chemist, where in their own professional interactions they choose to emphasize masculine attributes of ambition and self-interest rather than feminine attributes of compassion, cultivation, and contribution (Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018). This adaptability of gender performance to professional and personal contexts may have helped these women succeed in their careers. Further, these women had created their own niche in chemistry, or subfield (Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018). Authors of the investigation hypothesize that these subfields may afford a safe space free from gender-based pressures in which women “have the opportunity to simply be the women they are whilst doing the science they love” (Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018). Such subfields may be analogous to counterspaces – physical, ideological, or conceptual spaces that function to challenge deficit generalizations and provide a sense of belonging, connectedness, and validation for individuals with minoritized identities (Solorzano et al., 2000; Wade-Jaimes et al., 2021).
These strategies appear to be effective coping mechanisms for remaining in chemistry; however, they require women to either adapt their gender performance or restrict their activities to niche subfields, both of which perpetuate their minoritization. Thus, two questions remain if the discipline is to be truly inclusive of women: (1) How can women who wish to maintain their feminine attributes (e.g., ways of talking and acting) across professional and personal contexts successfully participate in the discipline? and more specifically, (2) How can these women maintain their feminine attributes and successfully participate in common disciplinary practices rather than specialized practices specific to emerging subfields of chemistry? Both of these actions will require identifying, disrupting, and reimagining exclusionary disciplinary norms rather than having women change or adapt so that they might adhere to these exclusionary norms. This work can begin by (1) identifying common disciplinary practices that prompt women's negotiation of self, (2) deconstructing norms surrounding these practices to understand their origin, how they have historically functioned to oppress individuals with minoritized identities, and systems of power that function to uphold them, and then (3) redefining these norms to be inclusive of all individuals. With regard to the second point, we note that our use of the term “minoritized” is intentional and distinct from the term “minority,” as it signifies that inequities are done to cause this position rather than it being inherently fixed (Kayumova and Dou, 2022).
Chemistry is characterized by masculine behavioral norms, which include acceptable ways of talking (Seymour and Hewitt, 1997; Eisenhart and Finkel, 1998). Moreover, a body of research documents the gendered nature of language (Lakoff, 1973; Areni and Sparks, 2005; Eckert and Mcconnell-Ginet, 2013), including differences between masculine and feminine communication styles. For instance, masculine communication is often marked by linguistic resources such as references to quantitative information (e.g., “6′4 tall”), judgmental adjectives (e.g., “good”), directives (e.g., “Write that down”), and “I” references (e.g., “I think…”), among others (Mulac et al., 2001). Conversely, feminine communication tends to be marked by linguistic resources such as intensive adverbs (e.g., “really” and “so”), references to affect (e.g., “happy”), uncertainty verbs (e.g., “It seems to be…”), hedges (e.g., “kind of”), and questions (e.g., “What's that?”), among others (Mulac et al., 2001). Self-identified men and women routinely employ a combination of masculine and feminine linguistic resources, with these groups often being more alike in their use of such resources than they are different (Eckert and Mcconnell-Ginet, 2013). However, differences can still be observed, where men tend toward masculine linguistic resources and women tend toward feminine linguistic resources (Mulac et al., 2001; Eckert and Mcconnell-Ginet, 2013). Further, the use of these resources serves as a means through which one constructs and performs their gender identity (Butler, 2011; Eckert and Mcconnell-Ginet, 2013).
Further, the gender order and linguistic norms within a community function to constrain one's linguistic practices (Lakoff, 1973; Eckert and Mcconnell-Ginet, 2013); this means that women may experience conflict in their linguistic practices when linguistic norms within a community are masculine, but they tend to use feminine linguistic resources to construct and perform their gender identity (Eckert and Mcconnell-Ginet, 2013). It is, thus, possible that communicating about NMR spectra is one disciplinary practice that requires women's negotiation of self, namely through the abandonment of feminine linguistic resources and increased adoption of masculine linguistic resources, if they are to form a chemistry identity. Such a possibility aligns with women's reported pressure to abandon their preferred feminine behaviors in exchange for masculine behaviors that characterize the discipline (Grunert and Bodner, 2011), negotiating their sense of self in order to be both a “woman” and a “chemist” (Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018).
Determining whether communicating about NMR spectra prompts women's negotiation of self is a productive first step toward identifying common disciplinary practices that require such a negotiation and, ultimately, deconstructing surrounding norms and redefining them to be inclusive of all individuals. In addition, chemistry education research documents the challenge that language poses to learning spectroscopy (Connor et al., 2021; Fantone et al., 2024). In particular, postsecondary organic chemistry instructors report that language constitutes a barrier to learning the practice of NMR spectral interpretation among undergraduates (Fantone et al., 2024). If communicating about NMR spectra does prompt women's negotiation of self, learning spectroscopy may be further complicated for these individuals, as women may need to simultaneously develop competence in this practice while also negotiating their sense of self to demonstrate, or perform, such competence to their peers and instructors. Moreover, communicating about NMR spectra is often done in English, with research documenting the unique barrier that language poses to learning spectroscopy among Eng+ students, or those who speak English as an additional language (Mundy et al., 2024). Thus, learning NMR spectroscopy may be even further complicated for Eng+ women, as it may require that they simultaneously develop competence and negotiate their sense of self, all while using an additional language.
NMR spectroscopy is included across the postsecondary chemistry curriculum (Connor and Raker, 2022). Therefore, understanding whether communicating about NMR spectra prompts women's negotiation of self may be important for equitably supporting learning throughout their chemistry education. To assist in these efforts, the study herein aims to provide initial insight into whether communicating about NMR spectra may prompt women's negotiation of self.
Gender performativity theory further posits that gender is maintained through repeated acts and performances, including gestures and speech, which are shaped by societal expectations (Butler, 2011; Li and Burkholder, 2024). Self-identified women are generally associated with a socially constructed set of attributes, behaviors, and roles termed femininity, and self-identified men are generally associated with a socially constructed set of attributes, behaviors, and roles termed masculinity (Li and Burkholder, 2024). Individuals with nonbinary identities often perform gender in ways outside of these associations. However, this theory also posits that individuals of all gender identities, including self-identified women, men, and nonbinary individuals, may exhibit fluidity in their performance of masculinity and femininity across different contexts. The study herein focuses on self-identified women and those comprising the outgroup in the context of the investigation – self-identified men. The gender performance of nonbinary individuals may variably align with those of this ingroup or the outgroup. Notably, however, STEM fields still minoritize people with nonbinary identities in distinct ways (Miller et al., 2021). Within this work, the terms women and men refer to self-identified women and men, respectively.
A number of variables contribute to one's vulnerability to experiencing social identity threat in a given context; among the most commonly investigated variables are stigma consciousness (Pinel, 1999, 2002, 2004; Brown and Pinel, 2003), domain identification (Steele and Aronson, 1995; Steele, 1997), and group identification (Schmader, 2002). General awareness of oneself as a member of a stigmatized social group in a setting in which the stigma is relevant is a preliminary condition to experiencing social identity threat (Casad et al., 2019), though the extent to which someone is acutely aware of or focused on one's stigmatized status in a setting increases their vulnerability to experiencing this threat (Pinel, 1999, 2002; Brown and Pinel, 2003). This acute awareness or focus on one's stigmatized status is termed stigma consciousness (Pinel, 2004); in other terms, stigma consciousness refers to the extent to which individuals believe their stigmatized status pervades interactions with members of the outgroup (Pinel, 2004). Gender stigma consciousness is, then, the extent to which one is acutely aware of or focused on stigma attached to their gender (Crocker and Major, 1989; Pinel, 2004). Domain identification refers to the extent to which one values the domain of interest, has requisite skills to succeed in it, and perceives it as being useful to their future career (Steele, 1997). Group identification refers to the extent to which one's membership in a social group (e.g., a gender identity) forms a central part of their self-concept (Phinney, 1992; Hoffman, 2006). For an individual who is knowingly a member of a stigmatized social group in a context where that stigma is relevant, their vulnerability to experiencing social identity threat increases with the extent to which they are acutely aware of the stigma surrounding the social group (Pinel, 2002; Brown and Pinel, 2003), value that domain and have requisite skills to succeed in it while also viewing the domain as important to their future success (Steele and Aronson, 1995; Steele, 1997), and hold membership in the stigmatized social group as part of their personal identity (Phinney, 1992; Hoffman, 2006).
Moreover, one's vulnerability to social identity threat is not necessarily inherent to the individual but, rather, can be modulated by situational cues (Murphy et al., 2007; Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008; Hall et al., 2018). Threatening situational cues are those that increase the saliency of one's stigmatized status and function to confirm one's potential for social devaluation based on this status (Casad et al., 2019), effectively increasing their vulnerability to social identity threat (Murphy et al., 2007). For instance, research suggests that for women in STEM, the numerical underrepresentation of women in a setting serves as one threatening situational cue that increases their vulnerability to social identity threat (Murphy et al., 2007). Research further suggests that these cues may specifically function to situationally-induce stigma consciousness, one variable contributing to an individual's vulnerability to social identity threat (Pinel, 2002; Brown and Pinel, 2003). Research on women's gender stigma consciousness, in particular, indicates that such consciousness can either be dispositional to an individual (i.e., trait consciousness) or situationally induced by cues which signal that a particular stigma is relevant in a given context (i.e., state consciousness). Moreover, research suggests that such cues may need to constitute strong situational factors in order to situationally-induce gender stigma consciousness among women (e.g., being presented with egregious examples of men discriminating against women prior to completing a stigma-relevant task; Pinel, 2004).
On the other hand, identity-safe cues are those that decrease the saliency of one's stigmatized status and function to disconfirm the relevance of a stigmatized identity (Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008; Casad et al., 2019), thereby creating a state where one's membership in a stigmatized social group is no longer an obstacle to success (i.e., a state of identity safety) and vulnerability to social identity threat decreases (Davies et al., 2005; Hall et al., 2018). Research identifies a number of strategies for creating identity-safe environments that reduce individuals’ vulnerability to social identity threat, including valuing diverse perspectives and approaches (Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008), affirming individuals’ sense of belonging (Walton and Cohen, 2007, 2011), and creating opportunities for individuals with a stigmatized identity to affirm their individuality (Ambady et al., 2004). For example, research demonstrates that women engineers experience less social identity threat when in organizations perceived to have more gender-inclusive policies (Hall et al., 2018). Research further demonstrates that black women in STEM environments with identity-safe cues experience greater trust and sense of belonging compared to environments without such cues (Pietri et al., 2018).
Identity negotiation theory further explores the role of personal identity and social group membership in the context of negotiation processes (Pruitt, 2001). Personal identity constitutes a representation of the self that distinguishes one from others (Brewer, 1991; Luhtanen and Crocker, 1992; Thoits and Virshup, 1997); it reflects one's set of attributes (e.g., personality characteristics and behaviors) that they consider to be individuating. Essentially, personal identity distinguishes an individual as a unique person (Chatman et al., 2005). However, membership in a social group (e.g., race, gender, socioeconomic status, profession, political affiliation, or religious affiliation) can hold deep meaning to an individual's sense of self and help distinguish themselves as a unique person, effectively becoming part of their set of attributes they consider individuating (Thoits and Virshup, 1997); in such instances, membership in the social group becomes part of one's personal identity. For example, an individual may view themselves as belonging to the gender category ‘woman,’ though being a woman may also hold deep meaning to their sense of self and serve to distinguish themselves from others. Likewise, an individual may view themselves as a member of the chemistry community of practice, where being a chemist may be similarly individuating. Further, an individual can create their own meaning surrounding membership in a social group rather than simply accept all its characteristic attributes when constructing their personal identity (Fordham, 1988). This notion accounts for observations that women tend to negotiate their sense of self in order to be both a “woman” and a “scientist,” a process that often involves redefining one's notion of what it means to be a “woman” or “scientist,” (Archer et al., 2012; Danielsson, 2012). Notably, however, this negotiation takes place in a masculine and heteronormative context where women who do not perform their gender in such ways are typically excluded (Danielsson, 2012; Calás et al., 2013; Avraamidou, 2020).
Identity negotiation processes take the form of either identity-enhancement strategies or identity-negation strategies (Deaux and Ethier, 1998). Identity-enhancement strategies function to enhance identity in response to threat and may include reaffirming a specific identity by ascribing increased importance to it, increasing contact with individuals who share the specific identity, or working to change social systems to allow for expression of the identity (Deaux and Ethier, 1998). On the other hand, identity-negation strategies function to negate identity and may include abandoning an identity or decreasing the importance of an identity without completing abandoning it (Deaux and Ethier, 1998). Code-switching serves as one such strategy, whereby individuals intentionally turn on and off certain attributes and behaviors associated with group membership (Cross and Strauss, 1998; Chatman et al., 2005). These latter strategies align with women's abandonment of feminine mannerisms and behaviors as they seek to create a unified sense of self as both a “woman” and “chemist” (Grunert and Bodner, 2011; Miller-Friedmann et al., 2018) and, more broadly, as a “woman” and “scientist” (Archer et al., 2012; Danielsson, 2012).
As one powerful example of the intersection of social identity threat theory and identity negotiation theory that underscores the importance of the study herein, one investigation found that women experiencing social identity threat regarding their leadership abilities reacted against the threat by adopting a more masculine communication style (i.e., a means of attaining identity fit in that context); (Hippel et al., 2011). However, women's adoption of a more masculine communication style ironically resulted in others rating them as less warm and likeable, with evaluators further indicating less willingness to comply with their requests (Hippel et al., 2011). Such results underscore the need for work focused on identifying, deconstructing, and redefining norms that are exclusionary of women in order to approach gender-inclusivity in the discipline.
Identity negotiation processes often occur in response to social identity threat (Deaux and Ethier, 1998; Chatman et al., 2005). Determining whether an individual experiences increased vulnerability to social identity threat when engaging in a disciplinary practice would, thus, serve as a proxy measure of these processes. Further, women's gender stigma consciousness (i.e., acute awareness of or focus on stigma attached to one's gender) contributes to their vulnerability to experiencing social identity threat in various contexts (Pinel, 1999, 2002, 2004; Brown and Pinel, 2003). Determining whether women experience increased gender stigma consciousness when engaging in a disciplinary practice would, thus, serve as preliminary evidence that the practice may prompt their negotiation of self. Women's increased awareness of stigma surrounding their gender when engaging in a practice would further suggest that women may perceive some difference between attributes of their gender and those comprising the outgroup, a perception which necessarily precedes one's negotiation of self (Deaux and Ethier, 1998).
An individual can experience situationally induced gender stigma consciousness in response to threating cues that signal a particular stigma is relevant in a given setting (Pinel, 2004). On the other hand, identity-safe cues function to reduce vulnerability to social identity threat by signaling that a particular stigma is irrelevant for success in the setting (Davies et al., 2005; Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008; Hall et al., 2018). For the study herein, simply participating in a disciplinary practice (i.e., communicating about NMR spectra) is investigated for potentially threating cues that may result in women's negotiation of self. To facilitate this investigation, this disciplinary practice is incorporated into a traditional task and a task with multiple identity-safe cues. Experiencing greater gender stigma consciousness on the traditional task versus the task with multiple identity-safe cues would then serve as evidence that women experience vulnerability to social identity threat when engaging in this disciplinary practice, as well as preliminary evidence that this practice may prompt women's negotiation of self.
1. To what degree is communicating about NMR spectra on a traditional task versus a task with identity-safe cues associated with greater gender stigma consciousness for women in postsecondary organic chemistry courses?
2. To what degree is gender stigma consciousness associated with women's performance or confidence when communicating about NMR spectra in postsecondary organic chemistry courses?
Findings from Research Question 1 provide insight into whether communicating about NMR spectra is a practice for which women experience increased awareness of stigma attached to their gender and are, in turn, vulnerable to social identity threat. Women exhibiting greater gender stigma consciousness on a traditional task versus one with identity-safe cues, in absence of the outgroup (i.e., men) exhibiting greater gender stigma consciousness, would constitute evidence of women's vulnerability to social identity threat while communicating about NMR spectra. It would further serve as preliminary evidence that communicating about NMR spectra may be a disciplinary practice that prompts women's negotiation of self. Findings from Research Question 1 will also help identify identity-safe situational cues that reduce women's vulnerability to social identity threat when communicating about NMR spectra and, in turn, provide insight into how norms surrounding this practice may be redefined to make it more inclusive. Findings from Research Question 2 will provide insight into the possible relationship between redefining norms surrounding this disciplinary practice and women's associated performance and confidence, providing a foundation through which future research can more deeply investigate causation.
Demographic variable | Percentage of study sample (%) |
---|---|
Gender | |
Woman | 65.2 |
Man | 28.5 |
Nonbinary | 3.3 |
Prefer to self-describe | 1.8 |
Prefer not to disclose | 1.1 |
Race | |
White | 52.2 |
Asian | 21.2 |
Black | 9.4 |
American Indian or Alaska Native | 1.7 |
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander | 0.2 |
More than one race | 12.0 |
Prefer not to disclose | 3.5 |
Ethnicity | |
Hispanic or Latino or Spanish Origin | 25.1 |
Not Hispanic or Latin or Spanish Origin | 72.1 |
Prefer not to disclose | 3.5 |
The university at which the study took place is a Predominantly White Institution, though the percent enrollment of Hispanic students approached the minimum requirement for federal designation as a Hispanic-Serving Institution (i.e., 25.0%) at the time of data collection. The percentage of participants in the study sample who identify as Hispanic, Latino, or of Spanish origin exceeds this minimum (see Table 1). The study was approved on August 19, 2021, by the Institutional Review Board of the university at which the study was conceptualized and data was collected and primarily analyzed (STUDY003120).
For the study herein, the SIAS was modified for use in postsecondary chemistry learning environments (i.e., the SIAS-Chem) and used to measure gender stigma consciousness. While the SIAS measures other constructs in addition to gender stigma consciousness that are not the primary focus of this investigation, modification and subsequent evaluation of the SIAS is part of a larger, ongoing investigation by the study team. To modify the SIAS for use in postsecondary chemistry learning environments, items referencing mathematics were revised to reference chemistry (see Appendix 1). These items were those that loaded onto factors corresponding to mathematics identification and negative mathematics affect. The modified SIAS-Chem thus included 30 items and six intended factors: Gender Stigma Consciousness, Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness, Gender Identification, Ethnic Identification, Chemistry Identification, and Negative Chemistry Affect (see Appendix 1). All items utilize a 7-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Item ordering was maintained during modification. Definitions of each subconstruct measured by the SIAS-Chem are modified from definitions used in studies on the original SIAS's development (Picho and Brown, 2011; Smith and Cokley, 2016) and provided below:
• Gender Stigma Consciousness – The extent to which one is aware of stigma attached to their gender.
• Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness – The extent to which one is aware of stigma attached to their ethnicity.
• Gender Identification – The extent to which an individual's gender forms a central part of their self-concept.
• Ethnic Identification – The extent to which an individual forms their identity based on membership within their ethnic group.
• Chemistry Identification – The extent to which an individual values chemistry, has requisite skills to succeed in the discipline, and perceives it as being useful to their future career.
• Negative Chemistry Affect – The extent to which an individual experiences negative feelings of dejection during chemistry test taking.
Factor | Item | Mean | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | Loading |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender Stigma Consciousness | My gender influences how teachers interpret my behavior | 3.63 | 1.92 | 0.08 | 1.88 | 0.71 |
Most people judge me on the basis of my gender | 3.28 | 1.81 | 0.35 | 2.14 | 0.78 | |
My gender affects how people treat me | 4.11 | 1.90 | −0.18 | 1.98 | 0.87 | |
My gender affects how people act towards me | 4.02 | 1.89 | −0.16 | 1.96 | 0.87 | |
Members of the opposite sex interpret my behavior based on my gender | 4.21 | 1.79 | −0.26 | 2.20 | 0.80 | |
Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness | Most people judge me on the basis of my ethnicity | 3.52 | 1.96 | 0.22 | 1.86 | 0.72 |
My ethnicity affects how my peers interact with me | 3.34 | 1.93 | 0.36 | 2.00 | 0.86 | |
My ethnicity influences how teachers interact with me | 2.90 | 1.77 | 0.60 | 2.36 | 0.84 | |
My ethnicity affects how I interact with people of other ethnicities | 3.28 | 1.99 | 0.37 | 1.87 | 0.77 | |
People from other ethnic groups interpret my behavior based on my ethnicity | 3.44 | 1.85 | 0.21 | 1.96 | 0.87 | |
Gender Identification | My gender influences how I feel about myself | 3.76 | 2.10 | 0.01 | 1.67 | 0.81 |
My gender contributes to my self confidence | 3.86 | 1.94 | −0.04 | 1.88 | 0.80 | |
My gender is central in defining who I am | 3.43 | 1.89 | 0.26 | 1.98 | 0.73 | |
My identity is strongly tied to my gender | 3.50 | 1.89 | 0.20 | 1.93 | 0.77 | |
Ethnic Identification | I value my ethnic background | 5.47 | 1.70 | −0.86 | 2.72 | 0.83 |
I feel a strong attachment to my ethnicity | 4.71 | 2.07 | −0.38 | 1.79 | 0.94 | |
My ethnicity is an important reflection of who I am | 4.49 | 2.08 | −0.31 | 1.79 | 0.89 | |
I am connected to my ethnic heritage | 4.54 | 2.10 | −0.32 | 1.76 | 0.91 | |
Chemistry Identification | Chemistry is important to me | 4.73 | 1.64 | −0.33 | 2.31 | 0.69 |
Being good at chemistry will be useful to me in my future career | 5.03 | 1.84 | −0.67 | 2.35 | 0.77 | |
My chemistry abilities are important to my academic success | 5.61 | 1.57 | −1.15 | 3.65 | 0.81 | |
Doing well in chemistry matters to me | 5.80 | 1.40 | −1.14 | 3.78 | 0.79 | |
I value chemistry | 5.20 | 1.60 | −0.62 | 2.63 | 0.79 | |
Doing well in chemistry is critical to my future success | 5.35 | 1.64 | −0.82 | 2.80 | 0.83 | |
Negative Chemistry Affect | When doing difficult chemistry problems on a test I… | |||||
Experience doubt about my chemistry abilities | 5.34 | 1.74 | −0.89 | 2.83 | 0.68 | |
Feel like I’m letting myself down | 5.04 | 1.90 | −0.67 | 2.28 | 0.82 | |
Start to lose confidence in my abilities | 5.08 | 1.87 | −0.71 | 2.36 | 0.83 | |
Feel like a failure | 4.40 | 2.17 | −0.23 | 1.62 | 0.94 | |
Feel hopeless | 4.22 | 2.22 | −0.11 | 1.56 | 0.92 | |
Feel like giving up | 4.05 | 2.24 | 0.02 | 1.53 | 0.84 |
CFA was conducted in RStudio using the “lavaan” package (Rosseel, 2012). To accommodate the non-normally distributed data, the robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator was used to estimate model parameters (Li, 2016). Model fit is evaluated using multiple statistics in CFA, including the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and χ2 statistic (Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). Conventional threshold values for acceptable model fit include CFI ≥ 0.90, TLI ≥ 0.90, RMSEA ≤ 0.08, and SRMR ≤ 0.10 (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006), and conventional threshold values for good model fit include CFI ≥ 0.95, TLI ≥ 0.95, RMSEA ≤ 0.06, and SRMR ≤ 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Worthington and Whittaker, 2006). These conventional threshold values are considered guidelines rather than strict rules (Marsh et al., 2004). Further, the χ2 statistic should be nonsignificant; however, this is unlikely due to the sample size.
Fit statistics from the six-factor correlated model exceeded the conventional criteria for good fit, where χ2 (n = 543, df = 384, p < 0.001) = 804.1, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.05, and SRMR = 0.04. Standardized item loadings ranged from 0.68 to 0.94 (see Table 2), and covariance coefficients ranged from 0.09 to 0.71 (see Table 10 in Appendix 2). Factor scores for each participant were generated using the CFA model.
With evidence of unidimensionality collected for five of the six factors, McDonald's ω coefficients were calculated using the “psych” package in RStudio (Revelle, 2018). Higher values approaching one are preferred and interpreted as being good (Guttman, 1945; Cronbach, 1951; McDonald, 1999). Coefficients approach one for the five unidimensional factors (see Table 3), providing evidence of reliability for those factors. The coefficient also approached one for the factor Gender Identification (see Table 3), though this measure may not provide meaningful evidence of reliability given that McDonald's ω is only valid for use with single-factor models.
Factor | ω |
---|---|
Gender Stigma Consciousness | 0.92 |
Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness | 0.92 |
Gender Identification | 0.91 |
Ethnic Identification | 0.95 |
Chemistry Identification | 0.94 |
Negative Chemistry Affect | 0.96 |
After collecting initial evidence of validity and reliability for the SIAS-Chem, measurement invariance was tested for the configural, metric, scalar, and conservative models comparing women by instructional prompt (i.e., Traditional Prompt or Identity-Safe Prompt) and men by instructional prompt (i.e., Traditional Prompt or Identity-Safe Prompt). Testing was conducted following the procedure in Rocabado et al. (2020), with the MLR estimator used to estimate model parameters. Significance was set at α = 0.05. The analysis was completed using the “lavaan” package in RStudio.
Conservative invariance was established for women by instructional prompt, as indicated by the continued data-model fit, lack of significant change in χ2, and lack of change in other fit statistics (i.e., CFI, SRMR, and RMSEA) when comparing the scalar and conservative models (i.e., the conservative testing level; see Table 4), along with the continued data-model fit and lack of change in data-model fit at all other testing levels (see Table 4). Conservative invariance implies that data from each group are sufficiently represented by the same factor model to facilitate meaningful comparisons of either factor scores generated from CFA or observed scale scores (Rocabado et al., 2020). Either factor scores or observed scale scores of women receiving the Traditional Prompt versus Identity-Safe Prompt can thus be meaningfully compared to address Research Question 1.
Testing level | χ 2 | df | p-Value | CFI | SRMR | RMSEA | Δχ2 | Δdf | p-Value | ΔCFI | ΔSRMR | ΔRMSEA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline (Traditional Prompt: n = 175) | 572.3 | 384 | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.05 | 0.06 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Baseline (Identity-Safe Prompt: n = 179) | 567.7 | 384 | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.05 | 0.05 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Configural | 1139.9 | 768 | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.05 | 0.05 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Metric | 1162.2 | 792 | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.06 | 0.05 | 22.3 | 24 | 0.561 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 |
Scalar | 1185.6 | 816 | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.06 | 0.05 | 23.4 | 24 | 0.496 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Conservative | 1198.5 | 846 | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.06 | 0.05 | 12.9 | 30 | 0.997 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Further, scalar invariance was established for men by instructional prompt, as indicated by the significant change in χ2 (Δχ2 = 48.6, Δdf = 30, p = 0.017) and change in CFI (ΔCFI = −0.01) when comparing the scalar and conservative models (i.e., the conservative testing level; see Table 5), along with the lack of change in data-model fit at all other testing levels (see Table 5). Scalar invariance implies that data from each group are sufficiently represented by the same factor model to facilitate meaningful comparisons of factor scores but not observed scale scores (Rocabado et al., 2020). The lower level of invariance may be due to the smaller number of men in the study sample. Nonetheless, factor scores between men receiving the Traditional Prompt versus Identity-Safe Prompt could still be meaningfully compared to address Research Question 1.
Testing level | χ 2 | df | p-Value | CFI | SRMR | RMSEA | Δχ2 | Δdf | p-Value | ΔCFI | ΔSRMR | ΔRMSEA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline (Traditional Prompt: n = 83) | 621.0 | 384 | 0.001 | 0.89 | 0.07 | 0.09 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Baseline (Identity-Safe Prompt: n = 72) | 614.0 | 384 | 0.001 | 0.88 | 0.08 | 0.09 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Configural | 1234.9 | 768 | 0.001 | 0.88 | 0.08 | 0.09 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Metric | 1261.1 | 792 | 0.001 | 0.88 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 26.2 | 24 | 0.292 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Scalar | 1296.9 | 816 | 0.001 | 0.88 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 35.8 | 24 | 0.057 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Conservative | 1345.5 | 846 | 0.001 | 0.87 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 48.6 | 30 | 0.017 | −0.01 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Both the conservative invariance established among women participants and the scalar invariance established among men participants provide support for comparing factor scores, though observed scale scores among women participants could also be meaningfully compared. In an effort to maintain consistency across analyses, factor scores from the SIAS-Chem are exclusively used for the investigation herein rather than observed scale scores. Factor scores are also a more precise measure than observed scale scores, as factor scores do not include item measurement errors (Putnick and Bornstein, 2016).
To address Research Question 2, participants’ total performance scores on the NMR-LRC were determined using the total number of correct responses, with possible scores ranging from 0.0 to 10.0. Total confidence scores on the NMR-LRC were determined using the average confidence score across NMR-LRC items, with possible scores ranging from 0.0 to 100.0. Descriptive statistics were then evaluated to summarize these distributions. Following descriptive statistics, Spearman's rank correlation coefficients (ρ) were calculated to evaluate correlations between gender stigma consciousness factor scores from the SIAS-Chem and total performance scores for women, as well as correlations between gender stigma consciousness factor scores from the SIAS-Chem and their total confidence scores. Bonferroni-adjusted significance was set at 0.008 (α = 0.05/6) to account for the increased likelihood of type I errors across the six tests used. Spearman's ρ is the nonparametric equivalent of Pearson's coefficient (Sheskin, 2011). While ρ < 0.1 is widely considered a weak relationship and ρ > 0.9 is widely considered a very strong relationship, there are no absolute cutoff criteria for intermediate values (Schober and Schwarte, 2018). All descriptive and inferential statistics were completed using Stata 17.0 (StataCorp, 2021).
Factor | z-Score | p-Value | Effect size (r) | Median (traditional prompt) | Median (identity-safe prompt) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Initial significance was set at α = 0.05, and the Bonferroni adjusted significance level was set at 0.008.a Corresponds to significance at p < 0.008. Small, medium, and large effects are approximated using values of 0.10, 0.30, and 0.50, respectively. | |||||
Gender Stigma Consciousness | 3.19 | 0.001a | 0.17 | 0.49 | 0.03 |
Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness | 2.43 | 0.015 | 0.13 | 0.19 | −0.29 |
Gender Identification | 1.58 | 0.115 | 0.08 | 0.27 | 0.28 |
Ethnic Identification | 1.61 | 0.107 | 0.09 | 0.46 | 0.26 |
Chemistry Identification | 0.50 | 0.620 | 0.03 | 0.21 | 0.01 |
Negative Chemistry Affect | 1.97 | 0.049 | 0.10 | 0.33 | 0.21 |
Further, there was no evidence of a difference in gender stigma consciousness factor scores across prompt types for men participants (see Table 7). Effect sizes corresponding to differences in chemistry identification and negative chemistry affect factor scores among men participants receiving the Traditional Prompt versus the Identity-Safe Prompt were substantive, despite p-values being insignificant (see Table 7); the sample size may, therefore, be too small to detect these differences. Thus, chemistry identification factor scores may be lower for men receiving the Traditional Prompt versus the Identity-Safe Prompt, and negative chemistry affect factor scores may be higher for men receiving the Traditional Prompt versus the Identity-Safe Prompt. There was no evidence of differences in factor scores between prompt types for the other measured constructs (see Table 7).
Factor | z-Score | p-Value | Effect size (r) | Median (traditional prompt) | Median (identity-safe prompt) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Initial significance was set at α = 0.05, and the Bonferroni adjusted significance level was set at 0.008. | |||||
Gender Stigma Consciousness | 0.56 | 0.580 | 0.04 | −0.29 | 0.04 |
Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness | 0.63 | 0.532 | 0.05 | −0.15 | −0.48 |
Gender Identification | 0.34 | 0.735 | 0.03 | −0.62 | −0.72 |
Ethnic Identification | 0.26 | 0.795 | 0.02 | 0.17 | −0.14 |
Chemistry Identification | 2.21 | 0.082 | 0.14 | −0.69 | −0.25 |
Negative Chemistry Affect | 1.74 | 0.027 | 0.18 | 0.50 | 0.00 |
Collectively, these results suggest that communicating about NMR spectra on a traditional task versus a task with identity-safe cues is associated with greater gender stigma consciousness for women in postsecondary organic chemistry courses. The difference in gender stigma consciousness factor scores among women participants provides primary evidence of this association, with the absence of such a difference among men participants (i.e., the majority outgroup) serving as supporting evidence. Further, the effect size of this difference among women participants is substantive, indicating this phenomenon is not negligible (Cohen, 1992). These findings further suggest that women experience vulnerability to social identity threat when communicating about NMR spectra. Because identity negotiation processes occur in response to social identity threat, communicating about NMR spectra may, therefore, be a common disciplinary practice that prompts women's negotiation of self. Moreover, the incorporation of identity-safe cues appears to reduce women's vulnerability to social identity threat when engaging in this practice. While these identity-safe cues do not serve to redefine exclusionary norms, they do help create a temporary space in which these norms may not apply. The reduction of women's vulnerability to social identity threat when in this temporary space thus serves as evidence that this practice could be made more inclusive through the redefining of surrounding norms.
Further, the substantive effect size corresponding to the difference in ethnicity stigma consciousness factor scores among women participants suggest that ethnically minoritized women who received the Traditional Prompt may have experienced greater ethnicity stigma consciousness (in addition to greater gender stigma consciousness) compared to ethnically minoritized women who received the Identity-Safe Prompt (see Table 6). The covariance coefficient (0.637, p < 0.001) between the gender stigma consciousness factor and ethnicity stigma consciousness factor on the SIAS-Chem was also moderate (see Table 10 in Appendix 2), suggesting these constructs were correlated for study participants. These findings align with prior research suggesting that ethnically minoritized women experiencing heightened awareness of stigma attached to their gender also experience heightened awareness of stigma attached to their ethnicity (Smith and Cokley, 2016). It is important to note that a substantive portion of the study sample identified as Hispanic, Latino, or of Spanish origin (see Table 1). Women with this doubly minoritized identity or others may, therefore, be at a heightened risk of experiencing social identity threat when communicating about NMR spectra. Moreover, identity-safe cues intended to reduce women's gender stigma consciousness when communicating about NMR spectra may have also functioned to reduce ethnically minoritized women's ethnicity stigma consciousness, pointing toward one potential strategy through which this practice could be made more inclusive for these individuals.
When considering the substantive effect sizes corresponding to differences in chemistry identification and negative chemistry affect factor scores among men participants, these results suggest that men receiving the Traditional Prompt versus the Identity-Safe Prompt may have identified less with chemistry while also experiencing greater negative feelings surrounding their chemistry test-taking abilities. The Traditional Prompt may, thus, also negatively impact factors related to men's domain identification. However, the use of identity-safe cues appears to reduce this negative impact among men in addition to reducing stigma consciousness among women, providing additional support for their use in tasks relating to NMR communication.
Variable | Mean | Median | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | Minimum score | Maximum score |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NMR-LRC total performance score | 5.2 | 5.0 | 1.4 | 0.05 | 2.91 | 2.0 | 9.0 |
NMR-LRC total confidence score | 59.2 | 61.0 | 16.4 | −0.69 | 3.96 | 0.0 | 97.5 |
Results from the evaluation of Spearman's rank correlation coefficients indicate that gender stigma consciousness factor scores are directly associated with NMR-LRC total confidence scores among women receiving the Identity-Safe Prompt (see Table 9). The size of this correlation coefficient (ρ = 0.215) was substantive, indicating the effect is not negligible. There was no evidence of an association between gender stigma consciousness factor scores and NMR-LRC total confidence scores among women receiving the Traditional Prompt (see Table 9). There was also no evidence of associations between gender stigma consciousness factor scores and NMR-LRC total performance scores regardless of prompt type (see Table 9).
Variables | Overall | Traditional prompt | Identity-safe prompt | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Correlation coefficient (ρ) | p-Value | Correlation coefficient (ρ) | p-Value | Correlation coefficient (ρ) | p-Value | |
Initial significance was set at α = 0.05, and the Bonferroni adjusted significance level was set at 0.008.a Corresponds to significance at p < 0.008. | ||||||
Gender stigma consciousness factor score – NMR-LRC total performance score | 0.030 | 0.573 | 0.039 | 0.607 | 0.007 | 0.930 |
Gender stigma consciousness factor score – NMR-LRC total confidence score | 0.133 | 0.012 | 0.063 | 0.411 | 0.215 | 0.004a |
Results from the evaluation of Spearman's rank correlation coefficients indicate that among women receiving the Identity-Safe Prompt, the more acutely aware one is of stigma attached to their gender, the more confident they are in their responses to NMR-LRC items. These results suggest that, in an identity-safe environment, women's gender stigma consciousness is associated with their confidence when communicating about NMR spectra. While this association does not provide evidence of causation, it is possible that receiving the Identity-Safe Prompt allows women with greater awareness of stigma attached to their gender to feel more confident in their responses, as they believe their responses will contribute to researchers’ understanding of the different ways that people (including women) communicate. This finding aligns with the observation by Miller-Friedmann et al. (2018) that women working in niche subfields of chemistry are able to flourish, possibly because they have a space free from gender-based pressures in which they can be themselves while doing something they enjoy. Thus, creating an identity-safe environment may not only function to minimize women's gender stigma consciousness when communicating about NMR spectra, but also increase confidence among women who still experience awareness of stigma when engaging in this practice. However, additional research is needed to understand the underlying cause of this association.
Other research studies have found that experiencing social identity threat when completing a task results in a decrease in associated performance; however, we found no evidence of an association between women's gender stigma consciousness (i.e., one measure of their vulnerability to social identity threat) and their performance on the NMR-LRC across prompt types. This lack of evidence for an association is somewhat encouraging, as it suggests that while women experience gender stigma consciousness when communicating about NMR spectra in postsecondary organic chemistry courses, awareness of this stigma may not impact their performance. Research suggests that one's performance may not be affected when experiencing social identity threat if the task is not difficult or if the individual does not strongly identify with the domain of interest (e.g., chemistry). Thus, women participants may not have found the NMR-LRC exceedingly difficult, as suggested by their score distribution (see Table 8). Given the introductory level of this course, it is also possible that the majority of women participants do not strongly identify with chemistry. Yet, regardless of the lack of evidence, experiencing acute awareness of stigma over time still undermines individuals’ sense of belonging and ultimately decreases their participation in associated domains (Froehlich et al., 2023). Therefore, lack of evidence for an association between gender stigma consciousness and performance scores does not detract from the need to redefine norms surrounding this practice.
Results suggest that women may have also experienced greater ethnicity stigma consciousness on the traditional task versus the identity-safe task, meaning that women with ethnically minoritized identities may be especially susceptible to experiencing social identity threat when communicating about NMR spectra in contexts without identity-safe cues. However, due to the quantitative and preliminary nature of this investigation, the lived experiences of women with intersectional identities were not explored. It is, thus, unclear from this investigation how these identities may interact and affect identity negotiation in the context of this disciplinary practice. Additional qualitative research will be needed to explore this intersectionality, an objective which is part of the study team's ongoing work. Complementary qualitative approaches would also be needed to understand the diverse experiences associated with gender identity. For instance, the extent to which findings apply to both cisgender and transgender women is unclear. This limitation is inherent when complex social constructs (e.g., gender stigma consciousness) are operationalized within exclusively quantitative frameworks. Nonetheless, the study team still views the quantitative findings herein as important evidence that communicating about NMR spectra is a disciplinary practice that may prompt women's negotiation of self; this evidence underscores the need for future investigations focused on other chemistry-specific practices and how their associated norms exclude certain social groups, and it serves as a foundation through which this work can be accomplished.
Further, the study occurred at a single, Predominantly White Institution with student population demographics that approached the minimum requirement for federal designation as a Hispanic-Serving Institution (i.e., 25.0%) at the time of data collection. The extent to which some findings, in particular the correlation between ethnicity stigma consciousness and gender stigma consciousness, may transfer to other instructional contexts is thus unclear. For instance, it is possible that this correlation is not observable at Predominately White Institutions where most students identify as non-Hispanic. The extent to which these findings transfer to other instructional contexts with different institutional demographics is also part of ongoing work by the study team.
With regard to the NMR communication task used for the investigation herein, it entailed written communication in an online setting rather than verbal communication in an in-person setting. This written design helped focus the investigation on women's gender stigma consciousness associated with communicating about NMR spectra in particular, as it served to minimize the effects of other contextual factors which may impact their stigma consciousness (e.g., being numerically underrepresented in a setting). However, NMR spectra are widely used tools for social discourse, where research documents their use in in-person contexts (Kozma and Russell, 2005). Additional research will, therefore, be needed to understand how in-person, verbal communication about NMR spectra in traditional settings versus identity-safe settings differentially impacts individuals’ gender stigma consciousness. The authors hypothesize that women's gender stigma consciousness could be exacerbated in traditional, in-person settings, as women may face additional threating situational cues that often accompany intergroup interactions (Pinel, 2002).
In addition, the investigation took place within an introductory-level instructional context rather than a professional context. There are inherent benefits to investigating this phenomenon at the introductory level. Namely, the likelihood that women have already undergone extensive identity negotiation processes to attain identity fit surrounding this practice in this early stage of their chemistry education is low, and if identity fit has already been realized, it would be difficult to determine if this practice prompts such negotiation. Further, evidence of women's increased gender stigma consciousness when participating in this practice at the introductory-level highlights how early identity negotiation processes may begin. However, additional research will be needed to understand how these findings transfer to professional settings.
Moreover, it is possible that women experience heightened vulnerability to social identity threat for other chemistry practices. Additional research will be needed to establish the extent to which such vulnerability extends to other practices. The SIAS-Chem, combined with traditional versus identity-safe prompts, serves as a promising tool for future identification of such practices. The SIAS-Chem could also be used to understand the general impact of traditional versus identity-safe chemistry learning environments on minoritized students’ susceptibility to social identity threat. However, additional evaluation of the SIAS-Chem in other instructional and institutional contexts is needed. Additional evaluation and potential refinement of the Gender Identification factor, which demonstrated questionable evidence of unidimensionality, is also needed. This evaluation is part of the study team's broader ongoing work, and readers interested in contributing to this evaluation are invited to contact the corresponding author.
Chemistry educators may also incorporate these strategies into their instruction on NMR spectroscopy. For example, they could begin by emphasizing that communicating about NMR spectra may take on valuable, diverse forms depending on an individual's background when initially introducing common terminology in the NMR lexicon (Connor et al., 2021). More transformational instructional practices could then involve actively encouraging and creating space for diverse forms of discourse during pre-laboratory and laboratory activities in which students engage with language surrounding spectroscopy (Mundy et al., 2024), both of which would serve to cultivate more inclusive disciplinary norms (albeit, in the classroom). Such practices will be important for achieving equitable learning outcomes in this topic. To avoid unconscious biases that persist within the discipline (Zhang, 2024), chemistry educators should also remain cognizant of the possibility that communicating about NMR spectra may take on different forms as they evaluate students’ competence surrounding this practice.
Further, language has been identified as a key barrier to learning spectroscopy (Connor et al., 2021; Fantone et al., 2024; Mundy et al., 2024), especially among Eng+ individuals (Mundy et al., 2024). Results of the study herein provide preliminary evidence that this barrier may also disproportionately impact women (and in particular, ethnically minoritized women), as they may need to simultaneously develop competence in this practice while also negotiating their sense of self to successfully perform this competence to their instructors. This possibility underscores the importance of creating identity-safe learning environments for this practice, as they will be needed to equitably support associated learning.
Further, measures of gender stigma consciousness were obtained using a version of the Social Identities and Attitudes Scale modified for use in chemistry learning environments (i.e., the SIAS-Chem), with evidence of validity, reliability, and measurement invariance across prompt groups providing initial, preliminary support for its use in identifying disciplinary practices that are exclusionary of women. However, the SIAS-Chem continues to undergo evaluation to support its broad use, both with regard to its Gender Identification factor and functionality in other instructional and institutional contexts.
Terms and concepts in some questions may not have been covered in your course, and so they may be unfamiliar to you. It's very important that we obtain natural responses from each student, so please respond without using outside information (classmates, textbook, internet, etc.). It's OK if you don't know the answer to something. All answers will be de-identified prior to analysis. Credit for participating will not rely on “correct” answers but rather an honest effort.
Terms and concepts in some questions may not have been covered in your course, and so they may be unfamiliar to you. It's very important that we obtain natural responses from each student, so please respond without using outside information (classmates, textbook, internet, etc.). It's OK if you don't know the answer to something. All answers will be de-identified prior to analysis. Credit for participating will not rely on “correct” answers but rather an honest effort.
Describe in full detail what specific features of the 1H NMR spectrum provide evidence that the synthesis of isochroman was successful or unsuccessful.
Explain in full detail why you think these specific features provide evidence that the synthesis of isochroman was successful or unsuccessful.
Example item from the 10-item NMR-LRC (Connor et al., 2021).
Strongly disagree | Strongly agree | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | |
My gender influences how I feel about myself | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Chemistry is important to me | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My gender contributes to my self confidence | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My gender influences how teachers interpret my behavior | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
I value my ethnic background | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Most people judge me on the basis of my ethnicity | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My gender is central in defining who I am | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Being good at chemistry will be useful to me in my future career | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Most people judge me on the basis of my gender | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My identity is strongly tied to my gender | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
I feel a strong attachment to my ethnicity | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My gender affects how people treat me | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My ethnicity is an important reflection of who I am | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
I am connected to my ethnic heritage | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My gender affects how people act towards me | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My chemistry abilities are important to my academic success | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My ethnicity affects how my peers interact with me | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Doing well in chemistry matters to me | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Members of the opposite sex interpret my behavior based on my gender | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My ethnicity influences how teachers interact with me | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
I value chemistry | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
My ethnicity affects how I interact with people of other ethnicities | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Doing well in chemistry is critical to my future success | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
People from other ethnic groups interpret my behavior based on my ethnicity | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
When doing difficult chemistry problems on a test I… | |||||||
Experience doubt about my chemistry abilities | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Feel like I’m letting myself down | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Start to lose confidence in my abilities | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Feel like a failure | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Feel hopeless | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Feel like giving up | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ | ○ |
Factor | Gender stigma consciousness | Ethnicity stigma consciousness | Gender identification | Ethnic identification | Chemistry identification |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness | 0.637 (p < 0.001) | — | — | — | — |
Gender Identification | 0.714 (p < 0.001) | 0.435 (p < 0.001) | — | — | — |
Ethnic Identification | 0.192 (p < 0.001) | 0.538 (p < 0.001) | 0.250 (p < 0.001) | — | — |
Chemistry Identification | 0.225 (p < 0.001) | 0.140 (p = 0.004) | 0.153 (p = 0.003) | 0.092 (p = 0.058) | — |
Negative Chemistry Affect | 0.197 (p < 0.001) | 0.160 (p = 0.001) | 0.137 (p = 0.007) | 0.160 (p = 0.001) | −0.074 (p = 0.144) |
Factor | χ 2 (df) | p-Value | CFI | TLI | RMSEA [90% confidence interval] | SRMR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender Stigma Consciousness | 6.52 (4) | 0.164 | 0.99 | 0.99 | 0.03 [0.01–0.07] | 0.01 |
Ethnicity Stigma Consciousness | 30.13 (5) | <0.001 | 0.98 | 0.96 | 0.10 [0.07–0.12] | 0.02 |
Gender Identification | 49.29 (2) | <0.001 | 0.94 | 0.82 | 0.21 [0.17–0.25] | 0.04 |
Ethnic Identification | 14.46 (2) | 0.001 | 0.95 | 0.98 | 0.11 [0.07–0.15] | 0.01 |
Chemistry Identification | 33.04 (8) | <0.001 | 0.98 | 0.96 | 0.08 [0.06–0.10] | 0.03 |
Negative Chemistry Affect | 91.86 (7) | <0.001 | 0.95 | 0.89 | 0.15 [0.13–0.17] | 0.03 |
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2025 |