Christine E.
Mundy
*a,
Marietjie
Potgieter
a and
Michael K.
Seery
b
aDepartment of Chemistry, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, Gauteng 0002, South Africa. E-mail: christine.mundy@up.ac.za
bCardiff Metropolitan University, Western Avenue, Cardiff CF5 2YB, UK
First published on 2nd April 2024
General spectroscopy is known to be difficult for novice students due to its complex and abstract nature. In this study we used a first-year chemistry Mini Spec laboratory activity to uncover language barriers to student learning in spectroscopy. Analysis revealed that language barriers generated conceptual difficulties for English as Second Language (ESL) students. As well as demonstrating difficulty with understanding of the origin of spectral lines identified in prior research, this work surfaces previously unreported language difficulties which were characterized in terms of technical and non-technical language. These include observations that ‘refract’ and ‘diffract’ appeared poorly delineated for students, the teleological animism of ‘jump’ to describe excited electron transitions towards the ground state, and the non-technical term ‘discrete’ being difficult for students to understand and construct meaning for. In addition to this, students battled with the symbolic language required to depict the formation of spectral lines. Several solutions to the language difficulty are proposed including the re-sequencing of macroscopic, sub-microscopic and symbolic teaching and reconsidering the usefulness of certain non-technical terms for teaching and learning spectroscopy.
When comparing an expert in chemistry and a novice, it is the ease of movement between these levels that defines proficiency in chemistry (Johnstone, 2010, Reid, 2021b). In essence, the multi-levels which are key to understanding chemistry are also what makes chemistry difficult to learn when they are encountered simultaneously. Laboratory learning provides an environment where all three levels come together, however, learning may be undermined if students are cognitively overloaded (Johnstone and Wham, 1982, Johnstone, 2006).
Similar challenges have been reported in physics and astronomy education. Bardar researched introductory college astronomy students’ understanding of light and spectroscopy concepts, reporting persistent difficulty even after (traditional) instruction for topics, such as the relationships between wavelength, frequency, and energy, as well as the connection between spectral features and underlying physical processes (Bardar, 2006, 2007). Related work on introductory physics students’ understanding of emission line spectra, energy levels, and transitions revealed several incorrect ideas about these topics and the relationships between them (Ivanjek et al., 2015). These included associating lines directly with individual energy levels (rather than a transition) and using incorrect models for emission of photons, such as the energy level itself “dropping”, rather than involving the electronic transition between levels.
The above work highlights two interrelated aspects that need to be considered regarding learning spectroscopy. The first is the conceptual understanding of spectroscopy. It is clear that there is a wide variety of alternative conceptions relating to understanding fundamental spectroscopy principles, and researchers and educators have sought to address these. The second considers how students transfer their understanding within a specific domain to other domains: for example, how they explain macroscopic observations using symbolic representations, and vice versa; and explicit in our context is the role of language capacity in achieving this goal. While work regarding conceptual understanding is well advanced, the impact of language capacity for novice learners – especially for those whom English is a second language – is less well understood. We aim to contribute to this understanding with this work. In doing so, we draw on two frameworks to underpin our study: Johnstone's triangle as already discussed as a means to document difficulties in understanding the topic of spectroscopy, and language literature to explore how these difficulties manifest for English as Second Language (ESL) learners.
English has become the universal language for communicating and building understanding in the sciences (Childs et al., 2015). Learning science when English is a second language (ESL) presents additional challenges: students may not have automatic access to meanings of words or extensive scientific vocabularies, and as such may have to rely on context to infer meaning (Mayer et al., 2014). Such construction of meaning creates additional cognitive load, which may overload the capacity of students who are most vulnerable (such as students who do not perform as well in assessments) (Fung and Yip, 2014, p. 1239). On the other hand, students may resort, or be coached, to learn vocabulary by heart. This rote learning or memorizing does reduce pressure on the working memory, and may be transient in the minds of the students as they are not able to assimilate the vocabulary into their long-term memory (Johnstone and Selepeng, 2001).
Whether students are ESL or first language speakers, proficiency in English is the key to conceptual understanding in science (Prinsloo et al., 2018). In fact, frequency of students’ use of, and proficiency in, language was the strongest predictor of performance in high school science in South Africa, surpassing economic backgrounds and available infrastructure at home such as electricity and water (Prinsloo et al., 2018). This complements earlier findings by Cassels and Johnstone (1984) that the complexity of language placed an unwelcome load on students, which affected their performance in multiple-choice items, even in the case where students were considered first language English speakers. Subsequent work documented that processing of language places particularly large demands on the working memory of ESL students (Johnstone and Selepeng, 2001). Kelly (2010, p. 5) outlines some of the stages of processing that ESL students face in chemistry that may add to load: “Learners may also need more opportunities to think about concepts in the foreign language as well as time to internalize the formal language, express it in their own words, and translate their own words back into the formal language of chemistry”.
We focus on the two main types of language terminology used: technical (T) and non-technical (NT) terminology as first described by Gardner (1972), expanded on by Oyoo (2007, 2017) and revisited in more detail by Quílez (2019). This classification tool for language difficulties will be used to frame the results of this study. In Table 1, the main distinctions and relevant types of technical and non-technical terminology are briefly explained with examples from emission spectroscopy in italics.
Technical terminology | Non-technical terminology |
---|---|
Defined broadly as “technical words or terminologies specific to a science subject; these may also be referred to as technical terms, scientific terms/terminology, science terms, or simply science words” (Oyoo, 2007, p. 232) | Defined broadly as everyday words that take on a scientific meaning when used in scientific disciplines (Oyoo, 2007). |
T1. Discipline specific e.g. photon, quantization | NT1. Non-technical terminology in scientific contexts e.g. excited |
These are either long standing or newly coined terms that were created deliberately within the discipline to describe new phenomena (Quílez, 2019). Due to the nature of science, the extent of scientific vocabulary is constantly growing. | These are common words to the language of instruction that take on a new, discipline-specific meaning when used as part of the language of science (Oyoo, 2017). |
T2. Terms with evolving meaning e.g. atom | NT2. Teleological terminology e.g. share |
The meaning of the word atom shifts with time or the meaning of the word changes based on the context, in this case the model of the atom in question at a particular time or for a particular application (Quílez, 2019). | The use of simplified terms that result in personification, animism or anthropomorphism in an attempt to improve student understanding (Taber and Watts, 1996, Quílez, 2019) |
T3. Symbolic language e.g. e−, λ, H, energy diagrams | |
Chemists use a specific symbolic language to explain the links between macroscopic observations and sub-microscopic representations (Taber, 2009, Rees et al., 2019) |
In our research, language came to the fore as a major barrier to students’ learning in spectroscopy. Spectroscopy is a complex and abstract topic, as such technical terminology was anticipated as a barrier for ESL students. Moreover, we sought to understand the facets of this barrier that were also non-technical in nature and thus challenging for an instructor teaching spectroscopy to anticipate. By combining the technical and non-technical terminology findings a substantial contribution may be made to pedagogical scientific language knowledge in this foundational topic.
• What language difficulties emerge for ESL students when explaining their conceptual understanding in introductory spectroscopy?
Based on our findings this work also intends to make recommendations for improving pedagogical scientific language knowledge in spectroscopy.
Data was collected over several cycles spanning three years. Design-based research was employed to fully support students’ laboratory learning and at the same time enabled the researchers’ to develop insights into the language barriers students may be facing (Mundy et al., 2024). The nature of the study's design allows for greater confidence in the findings presented.
General chemistry is a semester-long course in the first year, first semester of the academic development programme. This course has both a taught component, which is highly scaffolded and learner-centred, and a compulsory laboratory component. The study took place in one of six compulsory three hour practical sessions. Class sizes were typically 450 students, with students completing labs in cohorts of about 50.
Potential participants were purposefully approached by the demonstrators (senior students who assist in the lab) based on the time required to complete the Mini Spec laboratory experiment. Students were invited to participate from those who had finished early, those who finished comfortably within the time, and finally those who struggled to complete the exercise. This sampling technique was used to arrive at a representative cross-section of students using the pace of completion as a proxy for language proficiency and academic ability. Two sample groups were formed out of a total enrolment of 409 students, where n = 18 and n = 31. The only distinguishing factor between the groups was the just-in-time change of one term in one of the data collection tools.
Ethical clearance was granted by the corresponding author's institution for this research (180000144). All participants provided informed consent.
The Mini Spec is a simple device assembled from a template cut out of thick paper with a slit and a mounted wedge of CD to diffract incoming light. The Mini Spec was used to observe four types of light spectra, incandescent, sunlight, energy saver, and fluorescent. The Report Sheet guides students in exploring the components of the Mini Spec and recording observations, with the goal of building understanding of the significance of spectral phenomena.
In order to facilitate student preparation, pre-laboratory activities were incorporated into the process (Agustian and Seery, 2017). Before entering the laboratory, students prepared for the laboratory experiment using an instruction sheet, prescribed reading, and watching prescribed videos (Fig. 1, step 1). Students had to complete a pre-laboratory activity built into the Report Sheet before they could access the laboratory (Fig. 1, step 2). The completion of the pre-laboratory activity was verified by demonstrators. The demonstrators then led talks with students before the laboratory exercise commenced, this talk included relevant health and safety information, such as how to hold scissors for cutting complex pieces and how to safely observe sunlight (Fig. 1, step 3). Next students constructed their Mini Specs and submitted them to the demonstrators for verification before they begun to make observations (Fig. 1, step 5). After completing the laboratory exercise, students were expected to hand in their Report Sheets for grading upon exist of the laboratory.
Students’ Report Sheets were the second source of data collected. These Report Sheets contained five guided questions: the first two questions dealt with students’ understanding of the focusing slit and the diffractive grating in the Mini Spec. The third and fourth question were designed to make students explore the type of spectra observed and conceptualise what is meant by discrete and continuous spectra. The final question aimed to assess students’ understanding of the link between the brightness of spectral lines and their probability of occurrence. Students answered these questions during the laboratory and handed them in to be marked at the end of the session (see step 6, Fig. 1).
The third data source were the pre-laboratory and post-laboratory questionnaires. The pre-laboratory questionnaire was given to each student in the laboratory before they commenced work in the laboratory (see step 4, Fig. 1). The pre-laboratory questionnaire consisted of one multiple-choice question and an open-ended follow-up question asking students to give reasons for their choice. This two-tier approach was purposeful in its design as the follow-up question allowed students to construct their own explanations using technical or non-technical language, therefore language difficulties and their link to conceptual difficulties could be identified. The post-laboratory questionnaire again included the same multiple-choice question, and an open-ended follow-up question that asked students to give reasons for their current choice compared to their choice in the pre-laboratory questionnaire (see step 7, Fig. 1). Students were allowed as much time as required to complete the questionnaires; usually requiring five minutes.
The multiple-choice item which was chosen for this study represents a cumulative assessment tool proposed by Ivanjek, et al. (2015) after a large-scale study which sought to identify student difficulties with atomic emission spectra over four years with approximately 1000 participants (see below). The design of the item was purposeful in including known conceptual difficulties around the inter-related concepts of spectral lines, photon energies, and energy levels as distractors (Ivanjek et al., 2015) (Fig. 2).
The final data source came from recordings of students’ post-laboratory discussions. Post-laboratory discussions took place in small groups of 2 to 5 students. All students from sample 1 and 2 chose to participate. Students were conveniently allocated to groups by the laboratory demonstrator as they completed the laboratory exercise (see step 8, Fig. 1). The post-laboratory discussions were guided by a four question collaborative activity. The first two questions probed the “most important observation of”, and, “most important observation from” an energy saver globe. The third and fourth questions were a repeat of the post-laboratory questionnaire, the difference being that students should try arrive at a consensus instead of answering individually as in the pre- and post-laboratory questionnaire.
Reflexive coding using Atlas.ti was used to analyse the qualitative data from the Report Sheet and the written responses to the pre- and post-laboratory questionnaires, that is, the researcher constantly re-aligned the coding to the aims of the study whilst still allowing for insights to emerge. Initially the researcher immersed themselves in to data and came up with potential codes, revisiting and reflective on the codes was done in analytic coding memos used by the researcher to track the evolution of each code. This process resulted in a coding rubric in which students’ understanding was explored according to each question and the student responses were then coded as good, partial, and poor understanding of the concept at hand (see Tables 2 and 4). In creating the coding rubric, particular notice was given to explanations that suggested that student understanding was hindered by language difficulties. After two rounds of rubric refinement, excellent agreement was obtained between codes assigned by two independent coders (Spearman's rho rank correlation coefficient, rs > 0.839, p < 0.000, df = 39).
Code | Representative written responses | Frequency (n = 47a) |
---|---|---|
a Several responses (n) were not coded due to missing data or the quality of the photocopied hand-written Report Sheets. | ||
Diffraction, poor understanding | “Also, the shape of the CD was cut in to” P5 | 1 |
Students attribute a completely inappropriate purpose or phenomenon to the CD. | ||
Alternately, the perceived purpose of the CD may be just be the place where spectra may be viewed. | ||
Diffraction, partial understanding | “A piece of CD is similar to a prism because once light shines on it, it reflects the colour spectrum” P55 | 12 |
Students only acknowledge the reflective property of the CD not the diffractive purpose. | “Both the prism and the CD are light reflectors” P1 | |
Students may see light as bouncing off the surface of the CD, but do not acknowledge the interaction of light with the striated surface of the CD, causing the reflection of light at multiple angles or “diffraction”. | “It is similar because it reflects light and allows all or most colours to be seen” P31 | |
Note: reflection may have angles other than 180°. | ||
Diffraction, good understanding | “It diffracts and splits light into its component colours” P27 | 34 |
Students acknowledge that the beam is diffracted, split or spread out into its components when it interacts with the surface of the CD. | “It refracts the light in the same way that a prism does by separating the light into its different colours” P42 | |
“Defract” was acceptable as this appeared to be a common misspelling of diffract. | ||
“Refract” was only accepted based on the given explanation. |
A four quadrant descriptor system was used to quantitatively analyse the multiple-choice responses based on whether students answered correctly (C) or incorrectly (I): CC for students who were able to select the correct answer in both the pre- and post-laboratory questionnaire, IC for those who were incorrect in the pre-laboratory but corrected their answers post-lab, II for those students who were incorrect in their understanding of the formation of spectral lines both pre and post-lab; and, finally, CI, for those students who initially selected the correct answer but followed that with an incorrect selection in the post-laboratory questionnaire.
Representative vignettes of the recorded post-laboratory discussions are presented to triangulate findings from the questionnaires and the Report Sheets. The post-laboratory discussions served as an additional tool used by the researcher to gain insights into the linguistic barriers experienced by students in spectroscopy. The vignettes are given verbatim so that the reader can evaluate the strength of the claims made.
In the Report Sheet, a guided question was used to help students construct meaning for the purpose of CD and retrieve relevant discipline specific term, diffract, to describe this. The question, “How is the piece of CD in your Mini-Spec similar to a prism?” intended to prime the perception filter and stimulate feedback from the long-term memory. The phrasing of this question links the current Mini Spec laboratory experiment back to high school demonstrations and familiar illustrations of prisms splitting white light into rainbow colours. In Table 2, a response was coded as ‘Good understanding’ when a student used the discipline specific term, diffracts, or explained that the light from the source would be split or separated into its components.
The coding of responses was thought-provoking as many students used the word refracts in place of diffracts. Refraction is also a discipline specific technical term defined as the bending of light in different media such as the transition from water to air, or air to glass to air as in a prism. In the case of a prism, the refraction results in the splitting of light into its components but refraction does not always result in splitting. The wedge of CD diffracts the light into its components due to its striated (grooved or lined) surface. When analysing the students’ use of refracts in their written explanations, such as the response “the CD refracts the white light into seven different colours” (P29), the intended meaning of refracts coincided with diffracts, therefore such a response was agreed to be coded as ‘Good understanding’ even though it was not scientifically accurate (Table 2).
Reflect is a term that is non-technical, however, in a science setting it has a very specific meaning. If a student used this term to describe the purpose of the wedge of CD, the code ‘Partial understanding’ was used. The non-technical term reflect does not have any implication that there was an interaction of incoming light with the surface of the CD, just that there was a change of direction of the light.
A new term also emerged regularly in students’ explanations, defract. This term does not exist as either technical or non-technical. Having carefully checked the context of the responses, we conclude that ‘defract’ is a misspelling of the word diffract and thus a simple mistake, therefore it was coded as ‘Good understanding’.
Communicating the formation of spectral lines is often done using symbolic language, especially if a diagram is called for. In answering the question, students relied on what they were taught regarding the Bohr model of the hydrogen atom. Students needed to apply what they had learnt about spectral line formation in the hydrogen atom and realise that the ground state for the electrons in helium would be the same as for hydrogen (n = 1) as both elements only have electrons in the 1s orbital in the ground state. This often required referring to the periodic table and consulting with staff.
The majority of the students’ answers showed that the students drew sub-microscopic representations that reflected the principal energy levels in the atom along with symbols of possible downwards transitions that would result in six unique emission lines (see Fig. 3 and 4).
![]() | ||
Fig. 3 A student's response to the pre-lab activity (P51). Here the student acknowledged 8 possible transitions that would still result in only 6 emission lines. |
The diagram was supposed to be a means by which students communicate their understanding of the formation of spectral lines. Clearly, students incorrectly see the transitions or pathways as the emission lines themselves. Many students omitted electrons in their drawings along with photons, meaning it is unclear what is transitioning and what (if anything) is being released. Table 3 shows the frequency of symbols used in the diagrams, with almost all students showing downward transitions with an arrow. Of these 42 students, only 16 showed electron(s) on their diagram as spheres or using the more conventional symbol e−. This means that students either are unclear on the mechanism of spectral line formation or they do not invest in the accepted symbolic representations required to effectively communicate their understanding to a scientific audience. In fact, only 1 of the 47 students included both electrons and a photon in their diagram.
Symbol | Meaning | Frequency |
---|---|---|
a Several responses were not coded due to missing data or the quality of the photocopied hand-written Report Sheets. | ||
↑ | Upward transition (incorrect) | 5 |
↓ | Downward transition | 42 |
• | Electron | 13 |
e− | Electron | 3 |
![]() |
Photon | 2 |
Compare natural light (d) to the artificial light sources (a, b, c) in terms of the colours observed with your Mini Spec and the type of spectrum observed. Fully explain these findings.
Students’ responses to this question from samples 1 and 2 were coded as poor, partial, and good in their ability to both classify and explain their classification of the spectra (see Table 4).
Code | Representative written responses | Frequency (n = 46a) |
---|---|---|
a Several responses (n) were not coded due to missing data or the quality of the photocopied hand-written Report Sheets. | ||
Continuous vs. discrete, poor understanding | “Artificial lights can be used to replace a colour” P52 | 8 |
Students give an inappropriate response. | “The energy saver shows a continuous spectrum of light” P53 | |
Students may use the terms discrete or continuous, but there is no indication from their observations or discussion that the terms are used correctly. | “In the artificial light there were more dim array of colours indicating a decrease in kinetic energy” P19 | |
Students may also just repeat that sunlight produces a rainbow, out of context. | ||
Continuous vs. discrete, partial understanding | “Natural light contains all of the spectrum while artificial lines do not contain all of the colours” P17 | 32 |
Students use the terms discrete or continuous correctly to record their observations, but they do not elaborate on the meaning of the terms or may assign incorrect meaning. | “Artificial light contain a discrete spectrum” P27 | |
Students think discrete means the absence of some colours and do not see discrete as referring to definite bands. | “Natural light is continuous, 2 of the 3 artificial light sources are discrete” P42 | |
Continuous vs. discrete, good understanding | ““Natural light has a continuous spectrum – contains no boundaries, but incandescent is also continuous. Whereas artificial lights have a discrete spectrum which means that it has bands or boundaries” P21 | 6 |
Students show a good understanding of the classification of observations, even if they do not use the terms specifically. | “The artificial light sources displayed a discrete spectrum, which was like a colour separated by a dark solid line” P5 | |
Continuous is seen as “all” of the colours in the visible spectrum or a blend or blur of all of the colours. Discrete lines are described as lines or bands with dark spaces. |
Upon further investigation of students’ responses, it was revealed that many students classified as having partial understanding manifested an incorrect alternate conception of spectral line classification. Students saw continuous spectra to mean that all of the colours were visible, and discrete to mean that only some of the colours were visible in the spectra. This is not true, a discrete spectrum may contain all seven of the rainbow colours; the hallmark of a discrete spectrum is the banding of light, not the number of colours seen. Students appear to have constructed their own meaning for the terms continuous and discrete based upon a false dichotomy, for example, yellow emission lines are often missing in discrete spectra. Students’ constructed word-meaning and thus built concepts for themselves. These constructed word-meanings were flawed and do not allow for the next conceptual step: the revelation of quantized energy levels within the atom based on discrete light emissions for the excited electron.
This finding can be corroborated with the vignette from the collaborative post-laboratory activity below. Here it can be seen that Student 2 answers Student 1 with a flawed definition of discrete spectra based on flawed constructed word meaning. Student 3 tries to correct this but is unsuccessful as the Student 1 proceeds to build their misconception by conflating discrete or quantized spectra with the intensity of emissions within a spectrum. Student 3 does not attempt to correct the group again, and remains silent on this point.
Student 1: What is the difference between discrete and quantized?
Student 2: Discrete, it doesn’t show all the colours.
Student 3: No, it (quantized) has the same meaning as discrete
Student 1: Some bands are brighter than others, so they are quantized.
Post-laboratory | |||
---|---|---|---|
% correct (n) | % incorrect (n) | ||
Pre-laboratory | % correct (n) | CC | CI |
61% (11) | 6% (1) | ||
% incorrect (n) | IC | II | |
22% (4) | 11% (2) |
The four IC students exhibited partial understanding in the pre-laboratory questionnaire by acknowledging that transitions between levels result in emissions, but selected the photons as those transitioning (question option C), not the electrons (question option B). However, in the open item of the post-laboratory questionnaire, all four IC students from sample 1 acknowledged that they realised that it was the electron that must transition downwards for energy to be released.
Participant 48 written responses
Pre-laboratory: A photon is a package of light and as it passes between energy levels
Post-laboratory: When the electron moves down between energy levels, light is observed
Participant 57 written responses
Pre-laboratory: When a photon drops between different energy levels the energy increases. A photon gives up an energy level and energy increases (released)
Post-lab: My answer changed because I realised when an electron jumps a certain amount of energy levels down, it releases energy…
This is further corroborated by a vignette from a group discussion in sample 1:
Student 4: But it (the electron) is going to fall unless there is too much power to eject it. It is always going to fall .
Student 5: Okay it's going to fall. Let's say, if it receives energy, it is going to jump but then if it loses energy it is going to go back. So, this statement it says only jump. But you know an electron can lose energy and go back.
Student 4: But when it jumps it doesn't really emit anything does it?
Student 5: It does emit, some energy will be emitted.
Student 4: Because the energy increases as you go up?
Student 5: But then I think the photon drops because a photon is a pocket of light. So, whether it (the electron) jumps , the photon will alwaysdropbetween levels.
The simple non-technical terms jump or drop appear to have a large impact on students’ choices. A pre-occupation with the directionality of the teleological and animistic term jumps altered choices around the concepts of electron transitions and photon emissions. A decision was made to adjust the questionnaire wording for the repeat of the laboratory session from which sample 2 was drawn so that option B and C would become more similar for students: the word jumps was replaced with drops (see Fig. 5).
Six students from sample 2 fell into category II with most clinging to option D (see Table 6), making this a popular distractor and further affirming students’ difficulty with the concept of spectral line formation. Astonishingly, the decision to replace jumps with drops led to a higher prevalence of students choosing the correct option in the pre-laboratory questionnaire. This finding signposts the importance of language and terminology chosen by the instructor, even if it may appear as inconsequential.
Post-laboratory | |||
---|---|---|---|
% correct (n) | % incorrect (n) | ||
Pre-laboratory | % correct (n) | CC | CI |
74% (23) | 3% (1) | ||
% incorrect (n) | IC | II | |
3% (1) | 19% (6) |
The technical terminologies in this instance have additional difficulties in that they look similar in appearance, “look-alike”, and sound similar in pronunciation, “sound-alike” (Cassels and Johnstone, 1984, Oyoo, 2017). Additionally, these technical terms come from the same “word family” of the optics topic and therefore students may conflate their meaning (Rees et al., 2018), differing from words that merely “sound-alike” and “look-alike” but are unrelated in their meanings.
It is likely that students lacked appropriate prior knowledge to distinguish between (or apply) the terms of refraction and diffraction. This interpretation corresponds with “language fluency” referred to by Rees et al. (2019) where students require the language skills and conceptual foundations to discuss chemical phenomena at all levels of the chemistry triangle (Taber, 2013). A review of solutions details pedagogical strategies focusing on language and literacy to overcome difficulties with understanding (Oyoo, 2007). An appropriate future design decision may be to engage with the words more actively in the lectures and tutorials before students attempt the practical, or to build in a pre-laboratory activity that highlights the differences and similarities in these words (such as with the use of an animation or video).
By substituting the term “jumps” with the term “drops”, all the options in the multiple-choice read in a similar fashion and most students in the second sample were able to select the correct option both pre- and post-laboratory experience. As educators, instructors and designers of learning materials and classroom activities we tend to favour language that simplifies concepts for students. It may be beneficial to leave behind such animisms which lead to teleological reasoning in favour of technical terms like transition which have no directionality connotations for ESL students.
‘Discrete’ is the term frequently used to describe a spectrum consisting only of small or isolated segments of the electromagnetic spectrum (a line spectrum). The term “discrete” is no longer as common in the modern vocabulary as synonyms like separate or isolated. Furthermore, discrete is not as common in the modern vocabulary as its homophone, “discreet”, adding further strain to the cognitive scaffolding of the meaning of discrete in a scientific context. Additionally, instructors often equate the words discrete and quantized, this adds even more confusion in the mind of the learner, as the technical word quantized has no meaning outside of the scientific realm.
In this study, we tried to enable students to build their own definitions of the terms discrete and continuous, however, the lack of vocabulary knowledge around the non-technical term discrete allowed for students to form incorrect alternative conceptions of what the term discrete actually means. As the term discrete does not actually feature in the everyday modern vocabulary but it is still continued to be used in scientific communities, meaning that students have little or no prior knowledge to work with. If the word discrete has little meaning to ESL students in everyday use, students have to infer meaning from the laboratory exercise. This difficulty is a difficulty that is hidden from experts in the field who have either constructed their own schema for the meanings of such words or who have had the benefit of a more classical linguistic education of a bygone era. In striving for inclusivity in the scientific education arena, outdated everyday “non-technical” terminology will either need to be abandoned in the discourse, for example using the term separate or defined in place of discrete, or treated like a discipline-specific technical term which must be carefully explained by the practitioner. In terms of cognitive load, these propositions are very different: using modern everyday words may lower the processing demands on the students but may result in similar extraneous load caused by cognitive dissonance due to differing connotations of non-technical words for ESL speakers. Whereas introducing new scientific terminology will come with its own high intrinsic load on the working memory, due to its foreignness (Oyoo, 2007, 2017).
Understanding spectroscopy has foundations in high school topics of fundamental atomic structure, light and the electromagnetic spectrum. Tertiary instructors build on this knowledge between the topics to introduce spectroscopy at university level. It follows that high school teachers and tertiary instructors must speak the same language when teaching students in spectroscopy. To develop their pedagogical science language knowledge teacher/instructor must be aware of the dangers of simplifying language for ESL students and the load the simplified language places on ESL students. In addition to this, a glossary could be drawn up for teachers/instructors and students to use in preparation before beginning work on an abstract and complex topic like spectroscopy. Within the glossary, pertinent technical and non-technical terms should be described in detail.
The pre-laboratory activity, which prepares students to interpret their observations and make inferences, is an ideal place where students can engage with language relevant to the discourse in spectroscopy. In the pre-laboratory activity students could be guided to build meaning for the different terms, for example, an activity to link words with definitions, especially if those definitions are written in accessible language. We also propose providing students with a key or legend which links the terminology to symbols used often in the discourse would provide students with the tools to properly attempt the question, “…how many emission lines will be seen in the emission spectrum? Show your reasoning using an appropriate diagram”. Such a strategy should allow students greater proficiency in using a specialised language to manoeuvre between the levels of Johnstone's triangle in spectroscopy (Taber, 2013).
In this study we found that students communicate spectroscopic concepts poorest through symbolic representations. This finding has clear curriculum implications: experts have well developed schema in terms of spectroscopy and can easily navigate the macroscopic, symbolic, and sub-microscopic domains in this complex topic whereas students do not. Re-sequencing the symbolic representations to be the culminating taught component should help impart our expert schema to the students: “The use of sub-microscopic interpretations and the use of representations can be harnessed as ways to bring ideas together, provided that we do not work at too many levels at the same time with novice learners” Reid (2021a, p. 57). In spiralling the curriculum in this way, and starting with the macroscopic, as suggested by Johnstone (1991), we may be able to open doors for learner understanding of invisible mental models. A guided post-laboratory activity, in addition to an individual pre-laboratory activity, may be a solution in this particular context to consolidate understanding.
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2024 |