Dorian A.
Canelas
*a,
Jennifer L.
Hill
b and
Andrea
Novicki
c
aDepartment of Chemistry, Duke University, Box 90346, Durham, NC 27708-0346, USA. E-mail: dorian.canelas@duke.edu
bTrinity College Office of Assessment, Duke University, Box 104819, Durham, NC 27708, USA
cCenter for Instructional Technology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708-0198, USA
First published on 10th February 2017
Science and engineering educators and employers agree that students should graduate from college with expertise in their major subject area as well as the skills and competencies necessary for productive participation in diverse work environments. These competencies include problem-solving, communication, leadership, and collaboration, among others. Using a pseudo-experimental design, and employing a variety of data from exam scores, course evaluations, and student assessment of learning gains (SALG) surveys of key competencies, we compared the development of both chemistry content knowledge and transferable or generic skills among students enrolled in two types of large classes: a lecture-based format versus an interactive, constructive, cooperative learning (flipped classroom) format. Controlling for instructor, as well as laboratory and recitation content, students enrolled in the cooperative learning format reported higher learning gains than the control group in essential transferable skills and competency areas at the end of the term, and more growth in these areas over the course of the term. As a result of their work in the class, the two groups of students reported the most significant differences in their gains in the following areas: “interacting productively to solve problems with a diverse group of classmates,” “behaving as an effective leader,” “behaving as an effective teammate,” and “comfort level working with complex ideas.” Our findings clearly show that cooperative learning course designs allow students to practice and develop the transferable skills valued by employers.
Calls for colleges and universities to focus more on ensuring that science and engineering students are trained with parallel development of technical content knowledge and generic core competencies for the non-academic workplace have resounded for more than a decade (Kerr and Runquist, 2005; Martin et al., 2005; Shuman et al., 2005). Graduates cited feeling well-prepared for the technical aspects of their jobs – such as problem solving and readiness for life-long learning – but they also lamented weaknesses in formal training in terms of their readiness to effectively participate on teams or lead/manage interdisciplinary teams (Martin et al., 2005). Although this gap in transferable skills has been well documented for many years, researchers find that disparities continue to exist between targeted student competencies in the classroom environment and the full set of actual skills needed in technical professions. As an illustration, authors of a study focused on outcomes for undergraduate chemistry students note that “Some employers find chemistry graduates lacking written- and oral-communication skills, critical-thinking skills, group-work skills, as well as the ability to efficiently analyze data and retrieve chemical information” (Ashraf et al., 2011). Self-ratings of proficiencies reveal that the “ability to effectively work in a team” is a poorly rated skill in the college setting by students who have not yet started their careers (Baytiyeh, 2012). This skill is highly valued by both employers and students, but recent college graduates express greater confidence in their skills than employers report is warranted (Hart Research Associates, 2015). Perhaps most importantly, this same skill has the highest rating for necessity on the job (higher than all of the technical skill indicators) when ranked by those who are surveyed after they have been working for some time as technical professions (Baytiyeh, 2012). Similarly, recent chemistry graduates in the UK gave transferable skills higher “usefulness” rankings than chemical knowledge and skills (Hanson and Overton, 2010). Yet, when the same graduates were asked about the development of these transferable skills in their degree programs, they provided relatively low ratings for their development (Hanson and Overton, 2010). This assertion of a misalignment of the skills developed in the classroom setting and the interactive nature of true scientific discourse outside of the classroom environment is supported by a study that classified industrial chemists as transitional in their problem solving skills (Randles and Overton, 2015). Indeed, practicing chemists in industry occupy the process skills space between novice student learners and seasoned academics well-versed at open ended problem solving and intellectual discourse (Randles and Overton, 2015). A question then arises: can chemical educators improve the development of generic skills that industrial chemists need for high level functioning in the multidisciplinary teams often associated with the most complicated problem solving through revision of the curriculum or classroom environment?
The importance of generic or transferable skills has led to them being codified in the accreditation requirements for degree programs by organizations such as the Royal Society of Chemistry (RSC), American Chemical Society (ACS), and the Accreditation Board for Engineering and Technology (ABET) (ACS, 2015; ABET, 2016; RSC, 2016). As an illustration, for institutions to maintain accreditation of their bachelor's degree programs, RSC key requirements list transferable key skills for undergraduates such as “oral communication” and “an ability to interact with other people” (RSC, 2016). Similarly, the ACS committee on professional development's guidelines for bachelor's degree programs notes that institutions should provide “opportunities for students to learn to interact effectively in a group to solve scientific problems and work productively with a diverse group of peers” (ACS, 2015). The ABET expects programs to meet student learning criteria in three categories: technical, personal, and interpersonal skills (Baytiyeh, 2012). For the latter category, expected student outcomes include “An ability to communicate effectively with a range of audiences” and “An ability to function effectively on teams that establish goals, plan tasks, meet deadlines, and analyze risk and uncertainty” (ABET, 2016).
Since there is a mismatch in students’ perceptions of their own levels of competence with generic skills and employer reports (Hart Research Associates, 2015), what should undergraduate programs do to improve student outcomes in these areas? Some institutions endeavor to answer this question by developing a new course specifically focused on generic or transferable skills rather than on chemical content knowledge (Ashraf et al., 2011). Most chemistry departments, on the other hand, prefer to keep all courses that count towards the major focused primarily on technical skills and content knowledge. With this in mind, researchers have demonstrated that undergraduate science students can increase their proficiency in communication and teamwork skills through active learning pedagogies and curriculum innovation (Pontin et al., 1993; Bailey et al., 2012; Mittendorf and Cox, 2013; Whittington et al., 2014). Many science professors will point to the laboratory portions of courses as a place where skills such as collaboration and communication are practiced (Lowery Bretz et al., 2013). Indeed, laboratory experiences can be used and are often used to satisfy accreditation requirements for transferable skills. Moreover, laboratory classes have been explicitly shown to enhance development of soft skills for science students, especially when the instructor regularly and actively rotates the roles exercised by students in groups (Lategan, 2016). This is certainly a step in the right direction, but given the data reported by graduates of science and engineering programs, are laboratory experiences alone truly sufficient for developing students’ generic skills?
We aimed to aid and assess the development of both chemistry concept knowledge and students’ generic or transferable skills during their first year of college. We did this by employing inquiry-based, small group learning activities in an intentionally interactive, constructive, cooperative learning classroom format, and then comparing students’ learning outcomes in concept knowledge and generic skills to those of students enrolled in a lecture-based, or more passive, course. In this way, we could evaluate which types of knowledge and skills are most enhanced by the different types of classrooms.
This work is grounded in the ICAP theory (Chi, 2009; Chi and Wiley, 2014), which employs direct, overt observations of student behaviour during their learning opportunities as a proxy for varying levels of cognitive engagement. Observed behaviours are categorized into modes: Interactive, Constructive, Active, and/or Passive (ICAP). According to Chi and Wiley, “The ICAP hypothesis predicts that as students become more engaged with the learning materials, from passive to active to constructive to interactive, their learning will increase” (Chi and Wiley, 2014). Indeed, a large-scale, double-blind, randomized study supported the theory that student-centered, active-learning approaches improve science content learning when compared to teacher-centered approaches such as lecturing (Granger et al., 2012). While some recent studies have shown that organic chemistry content knowledge improves for undergraduate learners when they are in active learning classrooms as opposed to lectures (Hein, 2012; Conway, 2014), other studies have shown no differences in measured content knowledge outcomes (Dinan and Frydrychowski, 1995; Bradley et al., 2002; Chase et al., 2013; Rein and Brookes, 2015). We hypothesize that moving along the ICAP continuum toward the more interactive and constructive modes of learning in the classroom space will preserve acquisition of content learning while simultaneously improving learner confidence with important transferable skills such as teamwork, leadership, and collegiality.
To avoid confusion with terminology, and recognizing that frequently used words sometimes mean different things to different scholars, our use of language for describing the two types of classrooms in this study must be careful and deliberate. When describing our work, we will use the terms “control” and “lecture” fairly interchangeably. We will also use the word “experimental” and the phrase “cooperative learning” fairly interchangeably. In the latter case, the consistent language we have chosen here will be employed throughout the text to encompass the classroom format and/or students enrolled in the classroom format that involves the kinds of learning and activities that scholars in the field might refer to using many other words and phrases, such as student-centered, flipped, constructive, active learning, activity based, process oriented, inquiry guided, small group or team learning. We choose to clarify terminology here because we drew from a variety of traditions in developing the cooperative learning organic chemistry classroom format.
We present results from a case study that involves using the ICAP strategy for increasing student development of generic, transferable skills during the first semester of introductory organic chemistry in a large enrolment environment. Outcomes for students in the two cooperative learning sections were compared to outcomes for students in two traditional lecture sections of approximately the same size. Both formats were taught by the same instructor. Assessments of these student populations were conducted in three areas: psychological affect as a function of time (previously published) (Barger et al., 2015, 2017), formal summative assessments via assigned work and exams worth points in the course, and students’ self-reports of learning gains. For the latter purpose, which is the focus of this contribution, a Student Assessment of Learning Gains (SALG) survey instrument (Seymour et al., 2000; Jordan, 2012; Vishnumolakala et al., 2016) was developed and employed for comparison of the outcomes for students in the two types of classroom environments. Results for analyses of these surveys, student course evaluations through the established college system, and comparisons of results on graded course tests and assignments are presented herein.
All sections worked through the same amount of material: concepts covered in the first thirteen chapters of the required textbook for the course (Loudon, 2009). The only difference was how class time with the instructor of record was employed. More details about this difference are described in the following subsections. The overall structure of the gateway chemistry curriculum at the institution in this study have been previously published (Hall et al., 2014; Canelas, 2015; Goldwasser et al., 2016).
Numerous resources were available to students during their study time. Besides the instructor of record office hours, each graduate student teaching assistant was assigned to work two hours per week in the “Chemistry Resource Room.” The schedule for the Chemistry Resource Room was posted near the beginning of the term, and any student could drop in during any of the open times. The university also supported nine hours of evening and weekend “Walk-in Tutoring” which was staffed by undergraduates who had previously earned an A grade in the class. No appointments were needed for any of these resources, and attendance at these times for learning was not tracked by name on an individual student basis.
Various types of active, cooperative, small-group learning classroom environments for undergraduate organic chemistry have been previously reported in the literature (Dinan and Frydrychowski, 1995; Bradley et al., 2002; Tien et al., 2002; Hein, 2012; Chase et al., 2013; Conway, 2014; Fautch, 2015), so this work drew from the successes and lessons learned by examining the prior work. The instructor of record for the sections in this study had been teaching general chemistry classes in a cooperative learning classroom format with small, interactive student teams for several years, so basic challenges related to logistics and handling student teams during class time had been thoroughly addressed. In addition, a small section (36 students) using the cooperative learning format and group activities in this specific organic chemistry course was piloted by the instructor in the year before the experimental sections studied herein, so it was not the maiden voyage of the activities or classroom format for the course.
The observer sat in the back of class and noted what the instructor and students were doing during class every 5 minutes. These observations were converted to percentages of time spent in active learning. For the purposes of this study, “active learning” during class is defined as anything course-related that all students in a class session are instructed to do other than simply watching, listening and taking notes (Felder and Brent, 2009). For the current study, a time point was scored as active learning only when all students were engaged in a learning activity. Active learning results are expressed as a percent of the total observations.
For each survey item, the differences in the responses of the control and experimental groups were assessed by Student's t-tests; a p value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant. Reported numerical error values are the standard error of the mean (σM).
Characteristic | Control group | Experimental group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
n Students | % Students | n Students | % Students | |
Female | 169 | 63 | 176 | 59 |
Male | 101 | 37 | 120 | 40 |
Unknown gender | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 |
African-American | 34 | 13 | 39 | 13 |
American-Indian/Alaskan native | 3 | 1 | 1 | 0 |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 106 | 39 | 103 | 35 |
Caucasian | 100 | 37 | 121 | 41 |
Hispanic/Latino | 20 | 7 | 28 | 9 |
Native Hawaiian | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 |
Other race/ethnicity | 6 | 2 | 4 | 1 |
First generation | 24 | 9 | 31 | 10 |
Non-first generation | 246 | 91 | 266 | 90 |
Affirmative (yes) responses to the following items | Control group | Experimental group |
---|---|---|
% Students | % Students | |
a n ranges pre: control 219–224; expt. 254–261. b n ranges post: control 196–203; expt. 244–247. | ||
Baseline (pre) surveya | ||
I plan to major in chemistry (n = 479) | 13 | 18 |
I plan to attend a health-related professional school (medical, dental, vet, pharmacy, etc.) (n = 485) | 76 | 85 |
I plan to attend graduate school in the natural sciences (n = 476) | 37 | 42 |
I plan to gain employment as an engineer (n = 475) | 8 | 8 |
Post surveyb | ||
I plan to major in chemistry (n = 446) | 13 | 18 |
I plan to attend a health-related professional school (medical, dental, vet, pharmacy, etc.) (n = 450) | 76 | 80 |
I plan to attend graduate school in the natural sciences (n = 441) | 34 | 37 |
I plan to gain employment as an engineer (n = 442) | 10 | 9 |
The majority (76–85%) of the participants in both types of sections self-reported to be planning a pre-health course of study, indicating that they hoped to attend medical, dental, veterinary, or pharmacy school in the future. In these professions, which often involve extensive patient contact and require working in diverse teams of professionals, interpersonal skills are deemed especially important, and practice with these skills is considered an essential part of curricula (Makoul et al., 1998; Noble et al., 2007; Murdoch-Eaton and Whittle, 2012).
Approximately one third of the students answered “yes” to the item “I plan to attend graduate school in the natural sciences.” Apparently some of the students planned a future MD/PhD. The pre/post survey showed small declines (3–5%) in student future plans to attend health professional school or graduate school over the course of the semester. Eight to ten percent of the enrolled students planned to gain employment as an engineer.
Since both studies involved introductory organic chemistry courses, one reasonably might expect to observe the same four factors as Vishnumolakala et al. However, EFA was conducted for all of the questionnaire items concurrently, regardless of their likely categorization, thus producing a factor structure that is an authentic representation of the item associations and groupings that may be unique to this analytic sample. The present factor analysis was conducted using SAS Software, version 9.4, and specifying the promax option for oblique rotations (Cureton and D'Agostino, 1983; Gorsuch, 1983; Fabrigar et al., 1999), and it reveals three of the four constructs previously reported by Vishnumolakala et al.: process skills, concept learning, and active learning. The latter factor we have elected to describe using the term “interactive learning” herein (rather than active learning), since that phrase seems to fit our items better. We observed a fourth construct as well, but the questions loading onto that construct generally do not represent “resources”, as observed by Vishnumolakala et al. Rather, the pattern of factor loadings suggests that the fourth factor in this study represents “chemical modeling.” The labeling or categorization of factors requires interpretive discretion by the analyst, and given that many SALG questions could be attributed to multiple learning objectives, these deviations from Vishnumolakala et al.‘s findings are not unexpected. Table 3 displays the alignment of SALG items with each of the four observed factors, including the factor loadings (representing the strength of the association between the item and the observed factor).
SALG item number | Factor loadings | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Interactive learning | Concept learning | Chemical modeling | Process skills | |
1 | 0.88 | |||
2 | 0.87 | |||
3 | 0.86 | |||
4 | 0.82 | |||
5 | 0.79 | |||
6 | 0.72 | |||
7 | 0.65 | |||
8 | 0.71 | |||
9 | 0.71 | |||
10 | 0.71 | |||
11 | 0.72 | |||
12 | 0.73 | |||
13 | 0.76 | |||
14 | 0.76 | |||
15 | 0.77 | |||
16 | 0.77 | |||
17 | 0.77 | |||
18 | 0.78 | |||
19 | 0.79 | |||
20 | 0.80 | |||
21 | 0.82 | |||
22 | 0.82 | |||
23 | 0.83 | |||
24 | 0.85 | |||
25 | 0.86 | |||
26 | 0.71 | |||
27 | 0.78 | |||
28 | 0.78 | |||
29 | 0.82 | |||
30 | 0.83 | |||
31 | 0.57 | |||
32 | 0.62 | |||
33 | 0.64 | |||
34 | 0.66 | |||
35 | 0.67 | |||
36 | 0.67 | |||
37 | 0.69 | |||
38 | 0.69 | |||
39 | 0.70 | |||
40 | 0.70 | |||
41 | 0.71 | |||
42 | 0.71 | |||
43 | 0.71 | |||
44 | 0.71 | |||
45 | 0.71 | |||
46 | 0.71 | |||
47 | 0.72 | |||
48 | 0.72 | |||
49 | 0.74 | |||
50 | 0.74 | |||
51 | 0.75 | |||
52 | 0.75 | |||
53 | 0.76 | |||
54 | 0.77 | |||
55 | 0.77 | |||
56 | 0.78 | |||
57 | 0.79 |
To illustrate face validity (Nevo, 1985), Table 4 provides examples of the SALG items attributed to each of the factors/constructs. The questionnaire items loading onto each of the detected factors/constructs are reasonable and intuitive.
Factor | Examples of SALG items in that construct |
---|---|
Interactive learning |
How much did interacting with the instructor during class help your learning?
How much did working with peers during class help your learning? |
Concept learning |
Presently, I understand how to plan workable multi-step sequences of reactions that convert simple starting compounds into complex organic products using my knowledge of functional group reactions.
I can list several common synthesis methods for, as well as several common reactions of: alkenes. |
Process skills |
Presently, I can interact productively to solve problems with a diverse group of classmates.
Presently, I am in the habit of using systematic reasoning in my approach to problems. |
Chemical modeling |
Presently, I understand how to predict 3-D shapes and polarity of molecules.
Presently, I understand orbitals, including atomic, hybrid and molecular. |
Table 5 lists summary statistics that endorse the overall factor structure. Calculating percent of variance enables an estimation of the proportion of variance in the 57 survey items captured by the four selected factors. Cumulatively, the four constructs represented by these factors account for 77.5% of the variance. This high proportion likely is due to the large number of SALG questions loading onto the factors representing constructs “concept learning” and “process skills” (18 and 27 questions, respectively). Eigenvalues often are used to determine how many factors should be retained in the EFA. Because an eigenvalue of 1.0 means that the factor has the same explanatory power as a single variable in the analysis, higher eigenvalues represent greater explanatory power for that construct (Kaiser, 1960; Kim and Mueller, 1978). The factor representing the construct “process skills” has the most explanatory power, followed by “concept learning”. Cronbach's alpha (α) is a frequently employed and well-regarded estimate of internal reliability: the internal consistency among the questionnaire items attributed to the composite construct (Cronbach, 1951). Low alpha coefficients indicate high error, however the study results’ alpha coefficients all exceed 0.70, the commonly-accepted threshold for construct reliability (Nunnally, 1978). This study confirms, like Visnumolakala et al., that the SALG instrument measures essential, core learning objectives in undergraduate STEM education, and does so with a high degree of internal reliability and demonstrable construct validity.
Interactive learning | Concept learning | Chemical modeling | Process skills | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Percent variance | 12.55 | 24.65 | 9.88 | 30.42 |
Eigenvalue | 5.78 | 7.59 | 3.68 | 35.25 |
Cronbach's alpha (α) | 0.9521 | 0.9573 | 0.8806 | 0.9670 |
In spite of these large differences in the amount of time spent on passive versus active learning, students in both types of classroom formats performed similarly in terms of demonstrated application of content knowledge in chemical problem solving on the final exam. On a final exam with a maximum score of 300 points, the control group's mean score was 192.6 ± 2.8, while the experimental group's mean score was 191.8 ± 2.8 of (p = 0.846; for students who took the final exam: control group n = 258, experimental group n = 287). Indeed, no statistically significant difference was found when the overall final exam performance when these group were compared, and there were also no statistically significant differences in performances on the multiple choice portion of the exam. This finding is not surprising given that the exam focused on individual problem solving and content knowledge since the same content was provided to both groups. In addition, even the “lecture” format course employed a personal response system (clickers) and contained some shorter periods of intentionally interactive learning, so this could have contributed to the lack of difference in our summative assessment results.
These results confirm similar findings by several research groups who reported that changes in organic chemistry classroom formats did not significantly affect outcomes on summative assessments (Dinan and Frydrychowski, 1995; Bradley et al., 2002; Chase et al., 2013; Rein and Brookes, 2015). Interestingly, a pre- and post-test study for flipped versus lecture general chemistry classes (Reid, 2016) revealed that the “exam performance in the two sections is statistically different only for the bottom third, as measured by pretest score or percentile rank” (Ryan and Reid, 2016). Relatively weaker students were also found to benefit more than their stronger peers in terms of outcomes on tests in a flipped organic chemistry context (Fautch, 2015). On the other hand, our results offer a counterpoint to most other reports on college learning, which show a general trend of superior performance by students in flipped, inquiry-based, and/or other types of cooperative learning classroom environments (Freeman et al., 2014; Weaver and Sturtevant, 2015; Hibbard et al., 2016; Warfa, 2016). As an illustration, two recent studies specifically in organic chemistry revealed test performance benefits for learners in student-centered, active-learning organic chemistry classrooms (Hein, 2012; Conway, 2014).
Our study did not reveal a difference in exam performance for the groups in the two different conditions, perhaps because there were no real chemistry content knowledge learning differences, or perhaps because of confounding variables that mitigated any differences. On the latter point, even our passive, lecture classroom did include some active learning, which may have impacted the student outcomes for this condition on the summative assessment. In addition, our student population is from a highly selective university in terms of undergraduate admissions standards, similar to that reported by Bradley and coworkers (Bradley et al., 2002), who also did not see significant effects of classroom format on test scores. The interplay between students’ personal epistemologies and classroom formats may also have played a role, since students with authority-based justifications of knowledge tend to prefer and perform well in lecture format classes (Barger et al., 2015, 2017). Finally, the difference in format may have had a differential impact on a segment of our student population which was not revealed by our analysis. We did not examine the differential impact of active learning on different student populations (by demographics, previous grades, or pre-matriculation standardized test scores, for example), which may have revealed useful information similar to previous findings in studies which looked at student preparation distribution aspects in the analyses (Fautch, 2015; Reid, 2016; Ryan and Reid, 2016).
Results from the SALG survey items on course content knowledge gains are mixed. For each of the four factors (multi-item constructs), t-tests were used to explore possible differences in ratings between the groups, illustrated in Table 6. SALG items have a range of 1 to 5, where 5 is high. The analysis finds no statistically-significant overall differences in student self-evaluation of learning on items associated with the constructs chemical modeling and interactive learning but do show moderate and statistically-significant differences in the constructs concept learning and process skills (Table 6). Given the variety of learning outcomes included in the concept learning construct, and noting that not all questionnaire items associated with that construct show statistically significant differences across course formats, it is possible that the broad inclusivity of the SALG's concept learning construct masks specific, item-level learning outcomes. Item-level results presented in Table 7 suggest no statistically significant differences in a number of items attributed to the concept learning construct (such as how to draw the stereoisomers of chiral compounds, and how to identify the stereochemical relationship between structures) which may be better analytic matches for the performance tasks required by the course final exam. The analytic utility of Tables 6 and 7 are described in greater detail in the following section.
Control | Experimental | Difference | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Mean | σ M | n | Mean | σ M | ||
** Pr |t| < 0.01. * Pr |t| < 0.05. | |||||||
Interactive learning | 152 | 3.34 | 0.10 | 226 | 3.56 | 0.10 | 0.2206 |
Concept learning | 205 | 4.25 | 0.06 | 263 | 4.38 | 0.06 | 0.1339* |
Chemical modeling | 205 | 3.65 | 0.05 | 263 | 3.58 | 0.04 | −0.065 |
Process skills | 205 | 3.43 | 0.06 | 258 | 3.68 | 0.05 | 0.2527** |
Interactive learning | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
How much did each of the following aspects of the class help your learning? | Control group | Experimental group | Difference | ||
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
**** Pr |t| < 0.0001. *** Pr |t| < 0.001. ** Pr |t| < 0.01. * Pr |t| < 0.05. σM = standard error of the mean. | |||||
Interacting with the instructor during class | 3.18 | 0.14 | 3.61 | 0.11 | 0.434* |
Interacting with the instructor during office hours | 3.25 | 0.15 | 3.48 | 0.12 | 0.233 |
Working with teaching assistants during class | 2.66 | 0.20 | 3.20 | 0.15 | 0.543* |
Working with teaching assistants outside of class | 2.83 | 0.22 | 3.26 | 0.17 | 0.436 |
Working with peers during class | 2.72 | 0.25 | 3.10 | 0.20 | 0.383 |
Working with peers outside of class | 2.50 | 0.24 | 3.11 | 0.19 | 0.610* |
Walk-in tutoring | 3.37 | 0.13 | 3.69 | 0.10 | 0.318* |
Chemical modeling | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
As a result of your work in this class, what gains did you make in your understanding of each of the following? | Control group | Experimental group | Difference | ||
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
How to use resonance, when it is present, to predict the reactivity of a given compound | 3.67 | 0.08 | 3.63 | 0.07 | −0.041 |
How to draw Lewis dot structures | 3.56 | 0.09 | 3.42 | 0.08 | −0.142 |
How to assign formal charge | 3.83 | 0.08 | 3.65 | 0.08 | −0.177 |
How to predict 3-D shapes and polarity of molecules | 3.73 | 0.08 | 3.87 | 0.07 | 0.139 |
Orbitals, including atomic, hybrid and molecular | 3.45 | 0.08 | 3.35 | 0.07 | −0.101 |
Concept learning | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
As a result of your work in this class, what gains did you make in your understanding of each of the following? | Control group | Experimental group | Difference | ||
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
How to recognize the difference between conformations, constitutional isomers, and configurational isomers, including enantiomers | 4.27 | 0.06 | 4.38 | 0.05 | 0.106 |
How to identify chiral molecules | 4.44 | 0.05 | 4.52 | 0.04 | 0.088 |
How to draw the stereoisomers of chiral compounds | 4.43 | 0.05 | 4.45 | 0.05 | 0.013 |
How to identify the stereochemical relationship between structures | 4.32 | 0.05 | 4.34 | 0.05 | 0.016 |
How to explain the mechanisms of uni- and bimolecular substitutions and eliminations | 4.30 | 0.06 | 4.27 | 0.05 | −0.027 |
How to identify nucleophiles and electrophiles and predict reactions between them | 4.22 | 0.06 | 4.20 | 0.05 | −0.023 |
How to write a proper reaction mechanism for a given reaction with known products | 4.35 | 0.06 | 4.28 | 0.06 | −0.069 |
How to write mechanisms to predict outcomes for a half dozen types of addition reactions | 4.37 | 0.06 | 4.31 | 0.06 | −0.063 |
How to determine whether or not a reactive intermediate will rearrange its internal structure prior to further reaction | 4.29 | 0.06 | 4.25 | 0.06 | −0.043 |
How to identify all the functional groups present in a given organic chemical | 4.34 | 0.06 | 4.38 | 0.05 | 0.042 |
How to propose and predict the outcome of acid–base reactions | 4.17 | 0.06 | 4.04 | 0.06 | −0.134 |
How IR, NMR, and mass spectroscopy work & how I can use each kind of spectrum to identify structural facets of an unknown molecule | 4.12 | 0.07 | 4.32 | 0.05 | 0.196* |
How to plan workable multi-step sequences of reactions that convert simple starting compounds into complex organic products using my knowledge of functional group reactions | 4.01 | 0.07 | 4.31 | 0.05 | 0.304** |
(I can) list several common synthesis methods for, as well as several common reactions of: | |||||
Alkyl halides | 4.43 | 0.06 | 4.65 | 0.05 | 0.226** |
Alkenes | 4.44 | 0.07 | 4.75 | 0.05 | 0.307*** |
Alcohols | 4.38 | 0.07 | 4.58 | 0.06 | 0.206* |
Ketones and aldehydes | 4.08 | 0.07 | 4.29 | 0.06 | 0.203* |
Carboxylic acids and their derivatives | 3.99 | 0.08 | 4.30 | 0.06 | 0.304*** |
The biggest overall differences between the groups on the SALG post survey were the student responses to the following three items, all of which were prompted with “As a result of your work in this class, what gains did you make in the following skills?”
• Behaving as an effective team member
• Behaving as an effective leader
• Interacting productively to solve problems with a diverse group of classmates
All three of these items had highly statistically significant results in terms of differences observed between the groups (p < 0.0001 in all cases), with the experimental group reporting greater gains than the control group (Fig. 2 and Table 8). In contrast, skills that were not affected by the difference in classroom formats, such as skills related to the laboratory work, revealed no differences between the groups. For example, no statistically significant difference was found in participant's responses to the item “As a result of your work in this class, what gains did you make in the following skills: finding published data I need for lab work and report writing” (p = 0.6525). This item's lack of statistically significant differences between the groups completely makes sense, because all students were in the same format of laboratory. In fact, student assignment to a lab section had no correlation to the assignments in a specific classroom section of the course, so students from both groups were frequently in the same lab sections.
Process skills | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
As a result of your work in this class, what gains did you make in the following | Control group | Experimental group | Difference | ||
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
**** Pr |t| < 0.0001. *** Pr |t| < 0.001. ** Pr |t| < 0.01. * Pr |t| < 0.05. | |||||
Connecting key ideas and reasoning skills that I learn in my classes with other areas of my life | 3.13 | 0.09 | 3.46 | 0.08 | 0.324 |
Using systematic reasoning in my approach to problems | 3.73 | 0.08 | 3.97 | 0.07 | 0.244 |
Determining how each piece of new information or knowledge fits into the pattern of my existing knowledge | 3.83 | 0.08 | 4.09 | 0.06 | 0.261* |
Critically analyzing data and arguments before I make an action plan | 3.61 | 0.08 | 3.85 | 0.07 | 0.243* |
Developing testable hypotheses | 2.86 | 0.09 | 3.14 | 0.08 | 0.282* |
Designing and executing experiments | 3.10 | 0.10 | 3.26 | 0.08 | 0.157 |
Finding published data I need for lab work and report writing | 2.78 | 0.09 | 2.84 | 0.08 | 0.055 |
Critically evaluating debates in the media about issues related to science involving organic chemistry | 2.41 | 0.09 | 2.61 | 0.09 | 0.204 |
Identifying patterns in data | 3.30 | 0.08 | 3.25 | 0.08 | −0.048 |
Recognizing a sound argument and appropriate use of evidence | 2.89 | 0.09 | 3.05 | 0.08 | 0.159 |
Writing documents in the style and format appropriate for chemists | 3.07 | 0.09 | 3.27 | 0.08 | 0.202 |
Behaving as an effective team member | 3.22 | 0.09 | 3.94 | 0.07 | 0.713**** |
Behaving as an effective leader | 2.97 | 0.10 | 3.74 | 0.07 | 0.776**** |
Interacting productively to solve problems with a diverse group of classmates | 3.11 | 0.09 | 4.02 | 0.07 | 0.903**** |
Analyzing chemical models and draw appropriate conclusions | 3.83 | 0.08 | 4.08 | 0.06 | 0.246* |
Enthusiasm about learning organic chemistry | 3.55 | 0.09 | 3.97 | 0.07 | 0.413*** |
Your comfort level working with complex ideas | 3.66 | 0.08 | 4.08 | 0.06 | 0.410**** |
How much did each of the following aspects of the class help your learning? | Control group | Experimental group | Difference | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
Working on example problems given on the blackboard | 4.31 | 0.07 | 4.33 | 0.06 | 0.024 |
The number and spacing of tests | 3.78 | 0.08 | 3.84 | 0.08 | 0.052 |
The fit between class content and tests | 3.78 | 0.08 | 3.94 | 0.07 | 0.157 |
The mental stretch required by tests | 3.54 | 0.09 | 3.68 | 0.08 | 0.135 |
The feedback on my work received after tests or assignments | 3.41 | 0.09 | 3.68 | 0.08 | 0.276* |
Explanation of how the class activities, reading and assignments related to each other | 3.66 | 0.08 | 3.85 | 0.07 | 0.184 |
Explanation of why the class focused on the topics presented | 3.47 | 0.09 | 3.64 | 0.08 | 0.171 |
The instructional approach taken in this class | 3.95 | 0.08 | 3.70 | 0.09 | −0.242* |
How the class topics, activities, reading and assignments fit together | 3.85 | 0.08 | 4.02 | 0.07 | 0.168 |
The pace of the class | 3.18 | 0.09 | 3.33 | 0.09 | 0.143 |
Students in the experimental group expressed high levels of awareness that their interactions with peers, teaching assistants, and the instructor aided their learning (Table 7, Interactive Learning construct). Interestingly, interactions with peers outside of class was given higher ratings by students in the experimental section, and this result was statistically significant. Perhaps gaining comfort with classmates during in class activities led to improved student perception of transferable skills and richer out of classroom studying opportunities with peers? Dialog about scientific concepts and problem solving with peers and mentors serves as important practice for the process of scientific discourse and is crucial to the process for transitioning from a novice to an expert in a field. This is because discourse with peers allows learners to progress in the resolution of misconceptions (Kulatunga et al., 2013). This dialogical process is analogous to the professional practice of debating new ideas and explanations among practicing scientists (Osborne, 2010). In addition, peer discussions during complex problem solving both inside and outside of class provide important practice in the collegiality and teamwork needed in professional environments. Along these lines, Repice and coworkers observed small groups working to solve chemistry problems and note, “students engage in joint decision-making by taking turns, questioning and explaining, and building on one another's ideas” (Repice et al., 2016). Clearly, students in our experimental group could see the value in the practice of professional skills with peers in enhancing their learning.
When compared to the control group, students in the experimental group also assigned higher average ratings to helpful learning from working with teaching assistants during class. Recall from the description of the courses that all students (both control and experimental groups) had one discussion section per week led by a teaching assistant. Students in the experimental section gained additional classroom exposure to all of the teaching assistants, not just their own discussion section leader, because of their circulation during the active learning activities. So, it makes sense intuitively that the experimental group felt they learned more from the teaching assistants during class. Students in the experimental group also rated higher learning gains from the walk-in tutoring resource that was available to students in both groups. One explanation for this could be that exposure to a wider range of learning mentors during class time led to greater comfort levels with teaching assistants and peers, which in turn led to corresponding greater help seeking behaviour by the experimental group. Or, perhaps the experimental group just realized on a more regular basis that they were confused and not fully understanding all of the material in each class? We do not know, however, if students in the experimental group were more likely to attend the Chemistry Resource Room, walk-in tutoring, or instructor office hours.
Control group | Experimental group | Difference | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
**** Pr |t| < 0.0001. *** Pr |t| < 0.001. ** Pr |t| < 0.01. * Pr |t| < 0.05. | |||||
Quality of course | 4.24 | 0.03 | 4.13 | 0.05 | −0.109 |
Quality of instruction | 4.49 | 0.04 | 4.07 | 0.06 | −0.419**** |
Effort/work | 4.58 | 0.04 | 4.44 | 0.04 | −0.140* |
Difficulty of subject | 4.41 | 0.04 | 4.08 | 0.05 | −0.326**** |
Intellectual stimulation | 4.47 | 0.04 | 4.45 | 0.04 | −0.017 |
Instructor enthusiasm | 4.88 | 0.02 | 4.50 | 0.05 | −0.385**** |
Instructor access | 4.43 | 0.03 | 4.25 | 0.06 | −0.179** |
Participation encouraged | 4.37 | 0.04 | 4.15 | 0.07 | −0.224** |
Clarity of requirements | 4.58 | 0.04 | 3.93 | 0.08 | −0.651**** |
Quality of feedback | 4.08 | 0.04 | 4.10 | 0.07 | 0.022 |
Fairness of grading | 4.07 | 0.05 | 4.15 | 0.06 | 0.089 |
Progress toward | |||||
Factual knowledge | 4.40 | 0.04 | 4.37 | 0.05 | −0.024 |
Fundamental concepts | 4.41 | 0.04 | 4.40 | 0.05 | −0.011 |
Applying concepts | 4.33 | 0.04 | 4.32 | 0.05 | −0.016 |
Analyzing ideas and points of view | 3.30 | 0.06 | 3.27 | 0.09 | −0.030 |
Synthesizing knowledge | 4.33 | 0.04 | 4.22 | 0.06 | −0.104 |
Conducting inquiry w/ methods of the field | 3.87 | 0.05 | 3.69 | 0.08 | −0.179* |
Evaluate merit of ideas | 3.11 | 0.06 | 3.00 | 0.09 | −0.105 |
Oral expression skills | 2.28 | 0.06 | 2.94 | 0.09 | 0.656**** |
Writing skills | 2.52 | 0.06 | 2.92 | 0.08 | 0.400**** |
Time spent outside of class | 3.53 | 0.03 | 3.55 | 0.04 | 0.017 |
From the course evaluation written comments, very few students in the experimental group appeared to be fully aware that they were learning both organic chemistry and useful career skills. In fact, numerous students expressed hostility towards the cooperative learning classroom structure in their written comments, and these students appeared to believe that they could only learn by lecture. This could possibly be because the students are at an early stage in their college career, but it might also possibly arise from these individual students having a personal epistemology with an authority-based justification of knowledge (Barger et al., 2015, 2017). Such comments corroborate the numerical differences in perceptions of course quality, quality of instruction, instructor enthusiasm, and instructor accessibility. For example, one student commented:
It is extremely hard especially when you are in the flipped section and not the lecture section. There is never any actual instruction.
However, other course evaluation free response comments illustrated that some students in the experimental group did recognize that they were learning the course content at a deeper level, as well as gaining significant practice in important skills. For example, two different students commented:
Organic chemistry involved a lot more reasoning and critical thinking than I expected. This is a very logical course rather than just sheer memorization that most science courses (bio/general chemistry) tend to be.
The “backwards” classroom style definitely suited the course material. It promoted cooperation and problem-solving as opposed to memorization.
To evaluate whether there were notable differences in students’ perceptions of their skills, competencies and attitudes at the start of the term, which might influence our interpretations of post-test data, the study conducted t-tests on data collected via the baseline/pre-course SALG (Table 10). With respect to questions the EFA designates as interactive learning or process skills, there are no a priori differences in students’ self-reported competencies. The only statistically significant differences in baseline survey data occur among concept learning questions (see ESI†). Example items include:
Control group | Experimental group | Difference | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | σ M | Mean | σ M | ||
**** Pr |t| < 0.0001.*** Pr |t| < 0.001.** Pr |t| < 0.01.* Pr |t| < 0.05.σM = standard error of the mean. | |||||
Chemical modeling | |||||
How to use resonance, when it is present, to predict the reactivity of a given compound | 4.58 | 0.04 | 4.55 | 0.04 | −0.04 |
How to draw Lewis dot structures | 4.51 | 0.04 | 4.44 | 0.04 | −0.07 |
How to assign formal charge | 4.52 | 0.04 | 4.45 | 0.04 | −0.07 |
How to predict 3-D shapes and polarity of molecules | 3.83 | 0.06 | 3.78 | 0.05 | −0.05 |
Orbitals, including atomic, hybrid and molecular | 3.54 | 0.06 | 3.55 | 0.06 | 0.01 |
Concept learning | |||||
How to recognize the difference between conformations, constitutional isomers, and configurational isomers, including enantiomers | 2.46 | 0.07 | 2.26 | 0.06 | −0.19* |
How to identify chiral molecules | 1.40 | 0.06 | 1.42 | 0.06 | 0.01 |
How to draw the stereoisomers of chiral compounds | 1.42 | 0.06 | 1.37 | 0.06 | −0.04 |
How to identify the stereochemical relationship between structures | 1.70 | 0.07 | 1.43 | 0.06 | −0.28** |
How to explain the mechanisms of uni- and bimolecular substitutions and eliminations | 1.60 | 0.07 | 1.52 | 0.06 | −0.07 |
How to identify nucleophiles and electrophiles and predict reactions between them | 1.81 | 0.07 | 1.52 | 0.06 | −0.29** |
How to write a proper reaction mechanism for a given reaction with known products | 2.38 | 0.08 | 2.02 | 0.07 | −0.36*** |
How to write mechanisms to predict outcomes for a half dozen types of addition reactions | 1.79 | 0.07 | 1.63 | 0.07 | −0.17 |
How to identify all the functional groups present in a given organic chemical | 2.36 | 0.07 | 2.31 | 0.07 | −0.06 |
How to propose and predict the outcome of acid–base reactions | 2.54 | 0.07 | 2.43 | 0.06 | −0.11 |
How IR, NMR, and mass spectroscopy work & How I can use each kind of spectrum to identify structural facets of an unknown molecule | 1.47 | 0.06 | 1.56 | 0.06 | 0.09 |
How to plan workable multi-step sequences of reactions that convert simple starting compounds into complex organic products using my knowledge of functional group reactions | 1.57 | 0.06 | 1.48 | 0.06 | −0.09 |
(I can) list several common synthesis methods for, as well as several common reactions of: | |||||
Alkyl halides | 1.73 | 0.07 | 1.42 | 0.06 | −0.31*** |
Alkenes | 1.90 | 0.08 | 1.57 | 0.06 | −0.32*** |
Alcohols | 1.53 | 0.06 | 1.48 | 0.06 | −0.05 |
Ketones and aldehydes | 1.30 | 0.05 | 1.30 | 0.05 | 0.00 |
Carboxylic acids and their derivatives | 1.38 | 0.06 | 1.36 | 0.05 | −0.02 |
Process skills | |||||
Develop testable hypotheses | 3.36 | 0.06 | 3.46 | 0.06 | 0.10 |
Design and execute experiments | 3.18 | 0.07 | 3.35 | 0.06 | 0.17 |
Find published data I need for lab work and report writing | 3.22 | 0.07 | 3.28 | 0.07 | 0.06 |
Critically evaluate debates in the media about issues related to science involving organic chemistry | 2.57 | 0.07 | 2.60 | 0.07 | 0.03 |
Identify patterns in data | 3.48 | 0.06 | 3.43 | 0.06 | −0.05 |
Recognize a sound argument and appropriate use of evidence | 3.53 | 0.07 | 3.55 | 0.06 | 0.02 |
Write documents in the style and format appropriate for chemists | 2.94 | 0.07 | 3.01 | 0.07 | 0.08 |
Behave as an effective team member | 4.16 | 0.05 | 4.18 | 0.05 | 0.02 |
Behave as an effective leader | 3.90 | 0.06 | 4.05 | 0.05 | 0.15 |
Interact productively to solve problems with a diverse group of classmates | 3.95 | 0.06 | 4.03 | 0.05 | 0.08 |
Analyze chemical models and draw appropriate conclusions | 3.00 | 0.06 | 3.12 | 0.06 | 0.13 |
Enthusiastic about learning organic chemistry | 3.50 | 0.07 | 3.50 | 0.07 | 0.00 |
Comfortable working with complex ideas | 3.23 | 0.06 | 3.38 | 0.06 | 0.15 |
• how to recognize the difference between conformations, constitutional isomers, and configurational isomers, including enantiomers
• how to identify the stereochemical relationship between structures
In these instances, students in the experimental sections had lower mean self-ratings of concept mastery at the beginning of the course. That the differences in content learning are neutral (course final exams) or positive (select SALG items) suggests that the experimental pedagogy mitigates these differences and improves content learning over time. Likewise, the parity among course formats in interactive learning and perceived process skills at the beginning of the term, compared to the clear positive perceptions of learning outcomes for students in the experimental group (expressed by the SALG post-course data), suggests that cooperative learning is a viable innovation in undergraduate STEM education for reasons beyond gains in technical skills or content knowledge.
One may argue that the process skills evaluated by the SALG instrument represent essential 21st century learning objectives including critical thinking (Stein et al., 2007), information literacy, inquiry and analysis, problem-solving, and others (Maki, 2015). The SALG instrument also explores transferable skills accreditation requirements for degree programs by organizations such as the Royal Society of Chemistry (RSC), American Chemical Society (ACS), and the Accreditation Board for Engineering and Technology (ABET) (ACS, 2015; ABET, 2016; RSC, 2016). Innovations in pedagogy and curriculum design, as well as companion evaluative methodologies, are an essential response to these learning mandates, and the present study provides evidence of a workable approach to interactive, collaborative learning in an introductory science course. Without a doubt, flipped classrooms and other cooperative learning formats constitute viable innovations in undergraduate STEM education for important reasons beyond simply gains in technical skills or content knowledge.
This study was relatively small in scope and sample size and was limited to a single institution in an American educational context. The student population studied was fairly homogeneous both in terms of age and enrolment status: >90% of enrolled undergraduate students at the institution are traditional college age (under 24 years old) and carrying a full time course load. In addition, because the study was conducted at a highly selective institution in terms of undergraduate admissions, the students in this study may not constitute a representative sample of the global population of all students planning to pursue STEM or health professions. Most students in this study neither worked full-time during the academic year nor served as the primary caregiver to others. Due to the characteristics of the populations studied herein, caution must be exercised in attempting to extrapolate the findings to populations with more non-traditional students who are older, work full time, and/or serve as the primary caregiver to children or elderly parents. To protect individual confidentiality, and due to the modest number of participants, results in this study cannot be disaggregated by gender, race, ethnicity, or other demographic factors.
Footnote |
† Electronic supplementary information (ESI) available: Appendix 1: SALG baseline survey. Appendix 2: SALG post survey. See DOI: 10.1039/c7rp00014f |
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2017 |