Cynthia J.
Luxford
and
Stacey Lowery
Bretz
*
Miami University, Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Oxford, OH 45056, USA. E-mail: bretzsl@miamioh.edu
First published on 2nd April 2013
Chemistry students encounter a variety of terms, definitions, and classification schemes that many instructors expect students to memorize and be able to use. This research investigated students' descriptions of ionic and covalent bonding beyond definitions in order to explore students' knowledge about chemical bonding. Using Johnstone's Multiple Levels of Representation framework, an interactive interview protocol was designed to explore secondary and first year university chemistry students' understandings of bonding. This paper reports findings from the first phase of the interview when students were asked to create models to depict and explain covalent and ionic bonding using art supplies. Analysis of the student generated models and their accompanying explanations revealed multiple misconceptions about covalent and ionic bonding even though students could recite definitions.
Karl's descriptions of bonding ring true of the classic distinctions drawn by teachers when introducing bonding. In the textbook Chemistry: The Molecular Nature of Matter and Change(2006), Silberberg presents typical definitions for covalent and ionic bonding:
“Covalent bonding: The idealized bonding type that is based on localized electron-pair sharing between two atoms with little difference in their tendencies to lose or gain electrons (most commonly nonmetals)” p. G-5.
“Ionic bonding: The idealized type of bonding based on the attraction of oppositely charged ions that arise through electron transfer between atoms with large differences in their tendencies to lose or gain electrons (typically metals and nonmetals)” p. G-9.
Several other first year university chemistry textbooks present similar definitions (Brown et al., 2006; Millard, 2008; Tro, 2011). Such definitions are easily memorized by students. How do students understand these definitions? What ideas, if any, do they have about covalent and ionic bonding beyond these memorized definitions? How might chemistry teachers determine how students actually think about covalent and ionic bonding when such memorized definitions can be easily repeated verbatim?
Several other studies have described students' understandings of covalent bonding or ionic bonding through the use of student interviews (Taber, 1994, 1997, 1998; Schmidt, 1997; Boo, 1998; Coll and Taylor, 2001, 2002; Nicoll, 2001, 2003). These studies found that students' understandings of bonding include misconceptions about bond polarity (Peterson and Treagust, 1989; Coll and Taylor, 2001; Nicoll, 2001, 2003), the shapes of molecules (Peterson and Treagust, 1989), that covalent bond formation involves a transfer of electrons (Taber, 1994, 1997, 1998), that ionic bond formation involves a sharing of electrons (Coll and Taylor, 2001; Nicoll, 2001, 2003), and that the attraction between two oppositely charged ions can be attributed to neutralization (Schmidt, 1997; Boo, 1998). In Taber's work (1997, 1998), students clung to what he termed the ‘octet framework.’ They tended to explain bonding in terms of atoms ‘having,’ ‘needing,’ or ‘wanting’ eight electrons ‘in order to be stable.’
Some studies have used Lewis structures and molecular models to investigate student understanding of bonding. Nicoll (2001) used a series of tasks about the molecule formaldehyde. Students were asked to first define covalent bonding and then to draw a Lewis structure of formaldehyde given the formula COH2. Students were then asked to build a model using Playdoh to show what formaldehyde would look like, which revealed as yet previously unreported misconceptions. Coll and Treagust (2001) found that when undergraduate and postgraduate students were asked to describe or draw the bonding found in common items such as aluminum foil, both groups had a tendency to use simple teaching models learned in secondary school to explain chemical bonding. Cooper and colleagues have documented students' interpretations of Lewis structures (Cooper et al., 2010, 2012) and their extrapolation to properties that they believe are and are not encoded within such structures. Other research studies have shown that physical manipulative models may lead to confusion between ionic bonding and covalent bonding as learners believe a “stick” is an individual covalent bond (Butts and Smith, 1987; Birk and Kurtz, 1999).
There are no reports in the literature, however, regarding students building their own models for the express purpose of comparing and contrasting ionic bonding vs. covalent bonding. Given the importance of predicting properties from models (Cooper et al., 2012), it is important for chemistry instructors to know what students do and do not understand about the models used to depict bonding. This paper describes the methodology of using student-generated models and reports students' explanations of their models as well as analysis of the models and explanations for misconceptions. In particular, the students' models and their explanations are contrasted against their recitation of memorized definitions of covalent and ionic bonding typically provided in textbooks.
The focus of Phase 1 was to elicit students’ ideas without the influence of models typically generated by chemists. The remaining phases of the interview then asked students to interpret such expert representations. The findings reported herein focus on only the first phase of the interview, namely students' prior knowledge and the generation of their own models. Data and analyses stemming from expert generated representations will be published in future manuscripts.
Interviews were both audio and video recorded. The video was used to clarify students' comments during transcription, e.g., about ‘this’ atom or ‘that’ molecule. In order to protect students' identities, pseudonyms were assigned, and students' faces were not captured on video by purposefully directing the camera to capture student gestures (e.g., pointing to particular representations or moving models), but to avoid capturing facial expressions. Interviews were transcribed verbatim using both the audio and the video files. The models built by students were captured on camera. Subsequent screen shots were taken from the video in order to create image files of the models. All data were uploaded into NVivo 8 (QSR International Pty. Ltd., 2007), a data management software program to help with organization and retrieval of the coded interviews. The constant comparative method of data analysis was applied to the data (Strauss and Corbin, 1994, 1998; Taber, 2000). The interviews were first coded using line-by-line open coding, subsequently comparing open codes to one another, and then recoded using a more selective approach focusing on the specific themes that had emerged from the open coding (Lincoln and Guba, 1985; Guba and Lincoln, 1989; Strauss and Corbin, 1998; Patton, 2002; Creswell, 2007). In particular, the interviews were analyzed to examine how students discussed the models they built.
“Um covalent bonds involve one atom- er two atoms of elements sharing electrons and ionic bonds involve the attraction of charged particles. Um, covalent are typically two non-metals and ionic are typically a metal and nonmetal.” (Kevin, UC)
“Ionic bonding would be I guess where like one atom completely gets rid of an electron making it like… I think it would make it negative. And then it would give it to the other atom one extra electron making it positive… And when they bond covalently where you have two chlorine atoms and they both need one more so they just like… they include each other's one extra er one atom of each other's into the full circle.” (Kyle, SPS)
“Yeah like they can stand alone like ionic bonds I think are usually metals and covalent is like a metal and a nonmetal.” (Billie, SC)
From the data, the classification codes ‘sharing/giving up electrons’ and ‘metals and nonmetals’ emerged based on the commonalities across students as they themselves defined ionic and covalent bonding while building and explaining their models (Table 1). The ‘combined’ classification scheme was created for the five students who mentioned that in order to determine bond type, they would need to know both what atoms were involved and whether the electrons were shared or given away. Additional codes emerged from the data beyond the classification codes and were used to further expand our understanding of how students thought about their bonding models. Explanations invoking electronegativity differences, polarity, and the octet rule were of interest, but only three students discussed electronegativity differences and only two mentioned polarity while building their models. By contrast, 15 of the 24 students discussed the octet rule when describing their models.
Code | Code definition |
---|---|
Sharing/giving up electrons | • Describes/defines covalent and ionic bonding as one involving the sharing of electrons and the other involving transfer (giving up, getting rid of, gaining, etc.) of electrons. |
• May or may not discuss attraction of atoms rather than transfer. | |
• Might discuss the type of atoms (metals and nonmetals), however does not use the type of atom to determine or discuss the differences between the two bond types. | |
Metals and nonmetals | • Describes/defines covalent and ionic bonding based solely on the type of atoms being metals and nonmetals. |
• May discuss electrons as being either shared or given up, however does not use these features to determine the differences between the two bond types. | |
• Focus on periodic table trends (metals on left, nonmetals on right). | |
Combined | • Describes/defines covalent and ionic bonding based on the type of atoms being metals and nonmetals AND one bond type involving a sharing of electrons and the other bond type involving a transfer (giving up, getting rid of, gaining, etc.) of electrons. |
• Uses both sharing/giving up electrons and metals and nonmetals to classify the bond type as covalent or ionic. | |
Other | • Does not fit into the three categories above. |
• Does not mention atoms both sharing and giving up electrons, nor the type of atoms affecting the bond type. |
Each student was classified based on this coding scheme (Table 2). Inter-rater agreement was established with a second rater who analyzed 6 transcripts randomly selected from the corpus of 24. The classifications were discussed until 100% agreement was reached on these 6 students, and then the remaining 18 participants were coded once more. These categories were used to examine how student-created models and their descriptions thereof aligned (or not) with the definitions of bonding they had offered. Four definitions not fitting these categories, nor supported by textbooks, were grouped and labeled as ‘other definitions’. These four definitions ranged from a student admitting that she was just guessing to one describing each representation with regard to how the atoms in the representation were connected. It is important to note that ideas about bonding beyond the definitions of covalent bonding and ionic bonding emerged as students described their models. Students were categorized in Table 2 based on how they had defined covalent and ionic bonding before they built their models.
![]() | ||
Fig. 1 Examples of models created by students: (a) covalent ‘Ball and stick’ created by April (UC); (b) covalent Lewis structure drawn by Shelly (SPS); (c) ionic electron ‘orbits’ created by Lance (UC); (d) ionic lattice structure created by Nikki (UC). |
• ‘ball and stick’ models using Playdoh for atoms and toothpicks as connectors in order to show covalent bonding (Fig. 1a); ‘ball and stick’ model for ionic bonding by either removing the toothpick or by specifying a different meaning for the toothpick.
• Lewis structures for both covalent and ionic bonding (Fig. 1b).
• 3D electron models (orbits) including ‘electron orbits’ or ‘electron rings’ for both covalent and ionic bonding (Fig. 1c).
• Lattice structures in order to explain ionic bonding (Fig. 1d).
While most students built models contained within just one of these categories, seven students built a model in one category for ionic bonding and a different category of model for covalent bonding. Six students built multiple models for one type of bonding, and in doing so, used two or more categories to depict this one type of bonding.
A most interesting finding was that no direct relationship existed between the category of model(s) that a student built and the definitions that s/he offered for covalent and ionic bonding (Table 3). All the models in Fig. 1 were created by students who described and used the ‘sharing/giving up electrons’ classification scheme.
Sharing/giving up electrons | Metals and nonmetals | Combined | Other | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Covalent | Ionic | Covalent | Ionic | Covalent | Ionic | Covalent | Ionic | |
Model type | ||||||||
Ball and stick | 4 | 0 | 5 | 5 | 4 | 2 | 2 | 1 |
Lewis | 5 | 5 | 2 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Orbits | 2 | 2 | 1 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 1 | 1 |
Lattice | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
No model | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 2 |
Total | 12 | 10 | 8 | 6 | 8 | 5 | 4 | 4 |
Multiple models | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
# Participants | 9 | 6 | 5 | 4 |
Not only did students who offered the same definitions build different models, but as they described their models, some differing misconceptions about bonding also surfaced. Despite being able to ‘recite’ what they thought were standard definitions, students were confused about bonding. For example, one misconception that students held about ionic bonding was that electrons are first transferred and then they become like “a sea of electrons” in ionic compounds (Taber, 2003). Nikki (UC), who provided the sharing/giving up electrons definition for distinguishing between covalent and ionic bonding, built a 3D lattice structure (Fig. 1d) and offered this description:
“Ionic bonding is an array, like, repeating pattern and like the… it's the way we can make a sea of electrons. Electrons kind of like flow through everywhere. They are not being shared like between bonds. The electrons are free to move between each one.” (Nikki, UC).
Although Nikki had defined ionic bonding as “one atom giving up electrons” at the beginning of the interview, when she built a lattice model of NaCl to show ionic bonding, she described ionic bonding as electrons being able to flow from one atom to another. Nikki ignored the ideas of charges and the attractions between ions in her “sea of electrons” explanation. Without the prompts to generate the models and describe how they depicted bonding, this misconception would likely have gone undetected.
“The orange [Playdoh balls] are the Na… the Na plus molecu- er atoms and then the uh blue [Playdoh balls] is the Cl minus in NaCl and um there's like it would be it would be in a crystal form so um the charges of Na the positive charges. That's negative charged so their attracted to each other so they want to be as close as possible.” (Kevin, GC)
The other eight students described ionic bonding in terms of one atom giving or taking an electron from another. While the transfer of electrons is commonly discussed in textbooks to explain ion formation, all but one student described ionic bonding as the transfer or ‘giving up’ of electrons and not as the attraction between charged ions.
Daisy (SPS) provided definitions of sharing/giving and generated two models to explain her understanding. She initially drew a covalent Lewis structure for NaCl (Fig. 2a) and explained, “Chlorine has 7 electrons and sodium has just one, so to form sodium chloride I think that it, yeah, they share them like in the middle…” She then drew an ionic Lewis structure of NaCl (Fig. 2b) and explained, “That would be the taking one… ionic, where like sodium has just one so instead of… to get the perfect 8 instead of sharing they decide to just give it to chlorine.” Daisy thought that NaCl could be either ionic or covalent. When it was pointed out to her that she had drawn NaCl as both ionic and covalent and she was asked which it actually was, Daisy replied, “Either way, I guess they obviously can do it both ways.” Daisy did not understand electron transfer, nor the formation of charges and the attraction between charged ions to form the ionic compound structure.
![]() | ||
Fig. 2 Models created by Daisy (SPS) to show (a) covalent bonding and (b) ionic bonding. |
These excerpts from Daisy's interview highlight an important finding. While some students may be able to distinguish between sharing electrons and transferring electrons, these same students might not be able to determine whether two atoms will bonded to one another through the sharing of electrons or through ion formation and resulting attractions between ions. Daisy made no mention, for example, electronegativity differences that could be used to decide between the likelihood of covalent vs. ionic bonding. Rather she emphasized the importance of the explicit features of representations such as drawing overlapping circles in Fig. 2a or an arrow in Fig. 2b. To Daisy, the features she drew were what determined the type of bonding between two elements, even to the extent that the same two elements could either form an ionic bonded or a covalent bond to one another. She did not discuss any distinction between metals and nonmetals to help explain her representations. To her, the fact that each model was built using the same two elements was irrelevant. She invoked her prior knowledge of what meanings the arrow and lines conveyed and used these to build her models. Element identity, element symbols and metallic/nonmetallic properties were not critical features for Daisy. Her confusion about covalent and ionic bonding representations revealed that although she could generate some symbolisms used to depict bonding, she showed very little understanding of the ions and atoms that make up compounds.
Just as was the case with the students who offered definitions of ‘sharing/giving electrons,’ these students revealed misconceptions while explaining and discussing the specific features of their models, particularly with respect to the electrons in their representations. Diesel (SPS) said that both ionic bonding and covalent bonding involved the sharing of electrons. When asked how he would decide if a bond were ionic or covalent, he said:
“If you know the two types of atoms then two of those would be on the same side, that's ionic. And the one on the… if you have one from the right and one from the left side of the periodic table, that's how you know that the compound is covalent.” (Diesel, SPS)
Although Diesel contrasted the left side and right side of the periodic table, he had the locations of metals and nonmetals reversed. He did have an idea that one type of bonding takes place between a metal and nonmetal and the other between two nonmetals. Diesel then built a model that depicted a covalent bond as one electron being shared “to make eight valence electrons around an outer ring” (Fig. 3a). Curiously, the atom he built with blue playdoh had eight valence electrons and the yellow atom had only 3 electrons. Diesel's model for ionic bonding, however, also showed what he described as electrons being shared in order to make an ionic bond (Fig. 3b). He explained
![]() | ||
Fig. 3 Models created by Diesel (SPS) showing (a) covalent and (b) ionic bonding. |
“The electrons are both on the right side of the periodic table so they can't combine… equally so they are sharing from each to get the same amount of electrons.” (Diesel, SPS)
Diesel's models reveal his confusion about what constitutes a bond and how many electrons are involved. In his covalent model, one electron was shared and that one electron itself was the covalent bond. By contrast, the ionic model he generated used two electrons to make a bond, but he described the bond as the attraction being between the two electrons rather than an attraction between two oppositely charged ions. Diesel's ionic model conveys the idea that both atoms have 8 electrons, however he described the bond as being the toothpicks between the two electrons and talked about the electrons being attracted to one another. Despite having previously said that he would need to know what elements are being modeled, Diesel chose not to identify what elements his model was built from despite being asked to do so.
Billie (SC) generated two models for covalent bonding – first a Lewis structure of water and then a ball and stick model of water (Fig. 4). While the ball and stick model was built correctly (Fig. 4b), the Lewis structure lacked the correct number of electrons around oxygen and hydrogen (Fig. 4a) as she drew four electrons around each of the three atoms. Her model also revealed a lack of understanding about the nature of electron pairs, instead drawing them as single, unpaired electrons. Billie's models serve as an important caution for both teachers and researchers that asking students to generate only one type of model may not be sufficient to judge correct understanding and to reveal any misconceptions. If Billie had been asked to generate only the ball and stick model, she might never have struggled with constructing and interpreting the Lewis structure.
![]() | ||
Fig. 4 Models created by Billie (SC) showing covalent bonding in H2O through a (a) Lewis structure and (b) ball and stick model. |
Ryan (UC) built a space-filling model of ionic bonding by pushing two spheres of Playdoh together to make one round ball with 2 colored halves. While this model was a bit atypical in that the two ions were each half-spheres, Ryan commented that “together they are one compound, but if they are ions they can float apart.” He explained that what he meant by “float apart” was that if charges were present, the ions could separate from one another on their own without any other interactions. Indeed, he spoke of it as a spontaneous occurrence. He did not picture ion formation as happening until after the ions had “floated apart.” To Ryan, these ions neutralized one another in compounds with one positive ion and one negative ion coming together to form two neutral elements.
Vanessa (SPS) built multiple models to depict covalent bonding, but she chose to not assign identity to the elements. She first built a Lewis structure using generic Os for atoms and smaller circles to show where electrons would be placed (Fig. 5a). She shaded in seven of the electron locations and then further developed the Lewis structure into a 3D electron model using Playdoh (Fig. 5b). She built one atom with an orange nucleus and seven orange electrons. She built a second atom with a yellow nucleus and seven yellow electrons. She demonstrated what she meant by ‘sharing electrons’ by switching one of the orange electrons with one of the yellow electrons and then switching these same two electrons back again. She explained that in covalent bonding, sharing electrons meant that the two electrons “go back and forth” between the two atoms. As with Diesel, she viewed the bond to be an interaction between two specific electrons. She considered the other electrons to be stationary and not involved in the sharing. She did not mention whether these were the only electrons present in the atoms. Although Vanessa could correctly draw a Lewis structure and generate a 3-D model that resembled the Lewis structure, her description of the bonding electrons revealed that she did not understand that all electrons are in constant motion.
![]() | ||
Fig. 5 Models created by Vanessa (SPS) for covalent bonding using both a (a) Lewis structure and (b) 3D electron model. |
All but one student offered definitions for ionic bonding and for covalent bonding. Although a few students confused covalent bonding for ionic bonding and vice versa, they were able to articulate at least one difference between the two bond types. The most common definitions focused on covalent bonding as the sharing of electrons between two nonmetals and ionic bonding as the transfer of electrons from a metal to a nonmetal.
When students were asked to create their own models of ionic and covalent bonding, several important findings emerged that would not have come to light otherwise. First, there was no direct relationship between the category of models built and the definitions offered for covalent and ionic bonding. Second, misconceptions surfaced despite most students' ability to draw distinctions between metals and nonmetals or between sharing electrons and electron transfer. In addition to confirming several misconceptions already reported in the literature, asking students to generate their own models revealed that some students could not determine whether two atoms would share or transfer electrons when bonded to one another. While only one student discussed ionic bonding as attractions between ions, others described ionic bonds as the attraction between two electrons. Still other students considered the movement of particles included in their models to be an essential characteristic for depicting bonding. These misconceptions were found across multiple model types and definitions offered by the students.
The models also revealed that although students commonly recited a memorized definition for covalent bonding, these students thought of a covalent bond as one atom attracting the electron for a moment, then the other atom attracts the electron, and so on, back and forth between the two atoms. Any understanding of sharing as electrical interactions was missing from the students interviewed, and many of them, like Vanessa (HSPS), clearly showed with their models that they pictured bonding electrons as mobile while nonbonding electrons are not. Ionic bonding was not understood as electrical forces distributed through a lattice by most of the students interviewed. Only three students discussed ionic lattices and only one talked about attraction between ions. Instead, these students clung to the idea that ionic bonding was the actual physical transfer of electrons themselves.
Students created four different types of models: ball and stick, Lewis dot structures, electron orbits, and lattice structures. Overall, the models built across all three groups of students were very similar to each other, and the students held many of the same misconceptions and errors. Seven students spontaneously used more than one model to show one type of bonding in order to further explain additional characteristics of bonding beyond what their initial model showed. Students were encouraged to build additional models, but were not required to do so if they thought that their initial models were accurate.
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2013 |